You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

Subtle Differences in Cognition in 70-Year-Olds with Elevated Cerebrospinal Fluid Neurofilament Light and Neurogranin: A H70 Cross-Sectional Study

Abstract

Background:

Most research on cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) neurofilament light protein (NfL) as a marker for neurodegeneration and neurogranin (Ng) for synaptic dysfunction has largely focused on clinical cohorts rather than population-based samples.

Objective:

We hypothesized that increased CSF levels of NfL and Ng are associated with subtle cognitive deficits in cognitively unimpaired (CU) older adults.

Methods:

The sample was derived from the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies and comprised 258 CU 70-year-olds, with a Clinical Dementia Rating score of zero. All participants underwent extensive cognitive testing. CSF levels of NfL and Ng, as well as amyloid β1 - 42, total tau, and phosphorylated tau, were measured.

Results:

Participants with high CSF NfL performed worse in one memory-based test (Immediate recall, p = 0.013) and a language test (FAS, p = 0.016). Individuals with high CSF Ng performed worse on the memory-based test Supra Span (p = 0.035). When stratified according to CSF tau and Aβ42 concentrations, participants with high NfL and increased tau performed worse on a memory test than participants normal tau concentrations (Delayed recall, p = 0.003). In participants with high NfL, those with pathologic Aβ42 concentrations performed worse on the Delayed recall memory (p = 0.044). In the high Ng group, participants with pathological Aβ42 concentrations had lower MMSE scores (p = 0.027). However, in regression analysis we found no linear correlations between CSF NfL or CSF Ng in relation to cognitive tests when controlled for important co-variates.

Conclusion:

Markers of neurodegeneration and synaptic pathology might be associated with subtle signs of cognitive decline in a population-based sample of 70-year-olds.

INTRODUCTION

Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a common condition in older adults with normal cognition [1]. Preclinical AD is reflected in altered cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) AD biomarkers: amyloid-β (Aβ42), total-tau (t-tau), and phosphorylated tau (p-tau). We have previously investigated the performance on cognitive tests in cognitively normal older adults with amyloid and tau pathology (and noted only slight differences between cognitively unimpaired participants with preclinical AD pathology and those without [2]). In this study we sought to look into the performance of the same test battery in those with high concentrations of the biomarkers neurofilament light (NfL) and neurogranin (Ng). In previous research, it has been shown that cognitively normal individuals with underlying amyloid pathology are at greater risk to convert to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia [3]. As clinical trials for AD include cognitively unimpaired individuals with preclinical AD, it is important to understand if there are subtle cognitive differences present even in this very early disease phase and the relationship between cognition and the newer AD biomarkers NfL and Ng. These biomarkers reflect early pathological changes and have been shown to predict cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired older adults [4, 5].

NfL is an axonal structural protein, predominantly expressed in large caliber myelinated axons [6, 7]. NfL is released into CSF upon axonal damage and seems to be an unspecific marker as it is increased in AD and MCI [6], but also in other neurodegenerative conditions [8–10], and is associated with increased risk of progression to AD and worsening cognition [11]. CSF NfL was shown in one study to predict hippocampal atrophy rate in cognitively normal older adults, independently of other AD biomarkers [12], indicating that it could possibly play a role in affecting memory functions in the pre-symptomatic phase. However, it is still unclear if NfL correlates with subtle changes in cognition in the preclinical phase.

Ng is a post-synaptic protein, and its CSF levels reflect the degree of synaptic dysfunction in AD [13]. It is expressed in dendritic spines [14] and thought to be involved in memory functions [14, 15]. Ng is highly increased in the regions affected in AD: the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala [14]. Increased CSF Ng seems to be specific for AD [16, 17]. Some studies report that Ng is increased in early AD [4, 14, 18], which is consistent with autopsy and biopsy studies showing a considerable synapse loss early in the disease [19–21]. Although much uncertainty still exists about the relationship between synapse loss and neurodegeneration in AD, synaptic loss has been shown to correlate with cognitive decline (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Delayed recall) and may be the most reliable correlate of dementia [20, 22]. However, it is still unclear if Ng correlates with early changes in cognition.

Aims

Studies on NfL and Ng in cognitively normal older adults are sparse [4, 5]; to date, research has focused on clinical samples rather than population-based studies. We therefore aimed to investigate the hypothesis that high concentrations of NfL and Ng in CSF are associated with subtle cognitive deficits in cognitively unimpaired 70-year-olds. A secondary aim was to examine if such subtle cognitive differences were present in people with preclinical AD (pathologic CSF Aβ42 and/or tau concentrations) and high CSF NfL and/or Ng.

METHODS

Participants

The sample was part of the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort studies (the H70 studies) and was systematically derived from the Swedish Tax Agency’s population register. Seventy-year-old residents in Gothenburg born in 1944 on specific birth dates (ending with 0, 2, 5, or 8) were eligible for the study and invited to participate (n = 1,839) [23]. Of the 1,203 participants examined, 430 consented to a lumbar puncture (LP) (response rate 35.7%) but only 322 underwent LP due to contraindications in 108 (anticoagulant therapy, immune-modulating therapy, or cancer therapy). We based our analyses on a selection of people with a global Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) score of 0 [24] (n = 259) from these 322 individuals. After excluding six individuals with dementia, there were 313 individuals with NfL data and 315 individuals with Ng data. In the resulting CDR0 sample there were 256 persons with NfL and 258 with Ng data.

General examination

The examinations were conducted at the Psychiatry, Cognition and Old Age Psychiatry Outpatient Clinic at Sahlgrenska University Hospital in Gothenburg, Sweden. They included psychiatric and physical examinations, CSF sampling, blood-sampling for genetic analyses, examinations of social factors, diet, body composition, functional ability, MRI imaging, close informant interviews, and have been described in detail elsewhere [1, 23].

LP and CSF collection

LP was conducted by a neurologist or psychiatrist. Prior to LP, each participant underwent CT and/or MRI to detect contraindications [23], as described elsewhere [1]. CSF was collected by LP, conducted in the morning, from the L3/L4 or L4/L5 interspace, and transported immediately to the laboratory where it was centrifuged, aliquoted in polypropylene tubes and stored at –80°Celsius. CSF total tau and tau phosphorylated at threonine 181 (p-tau) levels were measured using a sandwich enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) (INNOTEST® htau Ag and PHOSPHO_TAU (181P), Fujirebio (formerly Innogenetics [25, 26].) CSF Aβ42 was measured using a sandwich ELISA (INNOTEST® β-amyloid1 - 42), specifically constructed to measure the 1–42 isoform of Aβ [27]. The following CSF cut-offs were used to define AD biomarker pathology [28, 29]: CSF Aβ42 concentrations ≤530 pg/mL, p-tau concentrations of ≥80 pg/mL, and CSF t-tau concentrations ≥350 pg/mL [1, 30].

CSF levels of t-tau, p-tau, and Aβ42 were analyzed as part of clinical routine diagnostics, using established procedures for quality control [31]. CSF aliquots were stored at –80°C pending analysis of NfL and Ng, which were analyzed using the same batch of reagents. CSF NfL [32] and neurogranin [14] were analyzed using in-house ELISA methods developed at the Mölndal Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory by board-certified laboratory technicians blinded to the clinical data. This procedure has been described in more detail previously [1, 23].

Cognitive examination

Examinations were performed by experienced research nurses, medical doctors, or a psychologist, and included ratings of psychiatric symptoms and signs, tests of mental functioning, including assessments of episodic memory (short-term, long-term), aphasia, apraxia, agnosia, executive functioning, and personality changes [1, 23].

Additional cognitive assessments were performed by a psychologist, research nurse, medical doctors, or trained research staff, using a neuropsychological test battery, including the following cognitive tests: 1) memory (Immediate and Delayed recall (12 object memory tests), Word memory (10 word memory list), Supra span (10 word memory list (BUSII) [33]), Thurstone’s picture memory test [34]; 2) language (semantic fluency animals, phonetic fluency controlled oral word association FAS); 3) executive function (Figure logic (SRB2), Digit span backwards); 4) visuospatial (Block design, (SRB3)); and 5) mental speed (Psif), all of which have been described elsewhere [23, 35, 36]. This battery covers the cognitive domains memory, language, visuospatial ability, executive function, and mental speed [2].

Global cognitive status was assessed by a Swedish version of the MMSE [37] and the assignment of a CDR score [24, 38], by research nurses or by a geriatric psychiatrist/neurologist [2].

Dementia was diagnosed according to the DSM-III-R [39] criteria as previously used in the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort studies, and major depression was diagnosed according to DSM-5 [40]. Education (defined as years of education) and history of stroke and transient ischemic attack (TIA) was acquired from self-reports and close informant interviews. Close informants and participants were also asked about family history of dementia, depression, and stroke [23].

APOE ɛ4 genotyping

The single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) rs7412 and rs429358 in APOE (gene map locus 19q13.2) were genotyped, using KASPar® PCR SNP genotyping system (LGC Genomics, Hoddesdon, Herts, UK). Genotype-data for these two SNPs were used to define ɛ2, ɛ3, and ɛ4 alleles [1]. Data on APOE genotype was lacking for five individuals.

Ethical considerations

All participants and/or their key informants provided written informed consent. The H70 study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Gothenburg and conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki [23].

Statistical analyses

Since the NfL variable was not normally distributed it was log transformed (log-10) prior to these analyses. This was also done with the Ng data, which was slightly skewed. Since there is no established pathological cutoff for CSF- NfL levels in unimpaired populations of older individuals and cut-offs are generally derived from patients with dementia, participants were divided into groups based on the median of CSF-NfL and CSF-Ng. Differences in sample characteristics were analyzed using Student’s T-tests for continuous variables and Fischer’s exact test for categorical variables. T-tests were performed to compare the results on cognitive tests between individuals with NfL above the median and NfL below the median. The same procedure was applied to analyze cognitive test results in relation to Ng. In addition, we compared neuropsychological test scores for participants with and without preclinical AD, using T-tests, in subsamples with high and low NfL levels. We used NfL and Ng as binary variables. Further, the cohort was divided into tertiles according to CSF-Ng and CSF-NfL concentrations, and we performed T-tests comparing cognitive test performance between the tertiles, with the bottom tertile as reference group. We also divided the participants according to amyloid and tau concentrations, where pathologic tau concentrations was defined as a combination of T-tau and P-tau in order to avoid small groups.

To adjust for potential covariates, linear regressions were performed with cognitive test performance as dependent variable, and NfL, age, education, sex, and APOE ɛ4 status as independent variables. The same procedure was repeated for the Ng variable.

A nonparametric sensitivity test (Mann-Whitney U-test) was also conducted comparing cognitive performance in individuals with NfL above the median with individuals with NfL below the median, and Ng above versus below the median. We also performed Hochberg correction for multiple testing and post-hoc power analyses.

A two-tailed level of significance was used for all analyses (p < 0.05). Statistical analyses were performed in IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (version. 25.0, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp), and Stata, version 14.0, StataCorp, Texas, USA. Normal distribution was assessed graphically and with Shapiro-Wilk’s test. The tau pathology variable contains some individuals with amyloid pathology and viceversa.

Power analysis

A post-hoc power analysis showed that we had a power of around 50–90%, for differences in means between groups of around 0.5 to 4 points on cognitive tests (i.e., the range of the effect sizes of the significant findings).

RESULTS

Sample characteristics

CSF-NfL and Ng concentrations in 70-year-olds with CDR0

Characteristics of participants (n = 258) are given in Table 1. Participants had a mean age of 70.6 (SD = 0.3) years, 50% were female and the mean educational length was 13.1 (SD = 3.9) years. Mean and median CSF NfL and Ng levels are given in Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics are presented by NfL and Ng status in Table 2, and part of this sample has been published before [2]. In participants with NfL concentrations, no differences were found between those with high versus low levels of NfL. The same applied for high versus low levels of Ng.

Table 1

Characteristics of study participants with Clinical Dementia Rating 0 (n = 258)

Characteristics
Women, n (%)129 (50)
Age, mean (SD), y70.6 (0.3)
MMSE score, mean (SD)29.3 (0.9)
Education, mean (SD), y13.1 (3.9)
Stroke, n (%)8 (3.1)
Any depression n (%)21 (8.1)
CSF-neurofilament light mean (pg/ml) (SD)915 (992)
CSF-neurofilament light median (pg/ml)724
Min: 276
Max: 12312
CSF-neurogranin mean (pg/ml) (SD)206.9 (73.3)
CSF-neurogranin median (pg/ml)195.7
Min: 71.7
Max: 513

A part of this sample has been published before [2].

Table 2

Characteristics of the study participants with Clinical Dementia Rating 0 by CSF Neurofilament Light and CSF Neurogranin status

Neurofilament lightNeurogranin
CharacteristicsBelowAbovepBelowAbovep
medianmedianmedianMedian
(N = 126)(N = 130)(N = 131)(N = 127)
Women, n (%)70 (55.6)58 (44.6)0.10463 (48.1)66 (52.0)0.618
Age, mean (SD), y70.5 (0.3)70.6 (0.2)0.09070.5 (0.2)70.6 (0.3)0.321
MMSE score, mean (SD)29.3 (0.9)29.2 (1.0)0.40029.3 (0.9)29.2 (1.0)0.566
Education, mean (SD), y13.2 (4.2)13.0 (3.6)0.75013.2 (3.9)13.0 (3.9)0.674
Stroke, n (%)4 (3.2)3 (2.3)0.7204 (3.1)4 (3.2)1.000
Any depression n (%)14 (11.1)7 (5.4)0.1138 (6.1)13 (10.2)0.260

CSF neurofilament light and CSF neurogranin are compared based on the median, the variables were log-transformed prior to T-test.

Cognitive performance in participants with CDR0 stratified by high versus low NfL and Ng concentrations

We then compared cognitive test performance between individuals with high versus low CSF NfL levels. Those with higher NfL levels performed worse on tests of memory (Immediate recall, 8.0 versus 8.5 p = 0.013, Cohen’s D = 0.31) and language (FAS, 39.9 versus 44.1 p = 0.016, Cohen’s D = 0.31) than those with lower NfL levels. No other differences in cognitive test performance were observed in participants with high CSF NfL compared to low CSF NfL (Table 3).

Table 3

Cognitive performance in participants with Clinical Dementia Rating 0, stratified by high versus low neurofilament light (NfL) and neurogranin (Ng) concentrations

Neurofilament lightNeurogranin
NfLMeanNfLMeanP1NgMeanNgMeanP2
belowscoreabovescorebelowscoreabovescore
median(SD)median(SD)median(SD)median(SD)
MMSE12829.3 (0.9)12729.2 (1.0)0.36412929.3 (0.9)12829.2 (1.0)0.706
Memory
Immediate recall1278.5 (1.5)1288.0 (1.7)0.0131298.3 (1.5)1288.1 (1.7)0.242
Delayed recall1277.9 (1.8)1287.5 (1.7)0.1141297.9 (1.7)1287.5 (1.7)0.126
Word memory1245.9 (1.7)1275.4 (1.8)0.0551275.6 (1.9)1265.6 (1.7)0.961
Supra Span (BUSII)1227.8 (1.5)1207.6 (1.4)0.2201227.9 (1.5)1227.5 (1.4)0.035
Thurstone’s picture memory test11722.8 (3.6)11922.8 (4.2)0.95311822.7 (3.9)11922.9 (3.8)0.613
Language
Word fluency12725.3 (6.7)12824.9 (6.3)0.66412925.4 (6.4)12824.8 (6.6)0.454
FAS12044.1 (14.4)12339.9 (12.9)0.01612341.9 (13.2)12242.0 (14.4)0.974
Executive function
SRB212420.8 (4.2)12620.3 (4.0)0.32512620.8 (4.3)12620.3 (3.9)0.357
Digit span backwards1254.5 (1.2)1254.7 (1.0)0.3351284.6 (1.2)1244.6 (1.1)0.809
Visuospatial
SRB312122.3 (6.5)12521.7 (7.1)0.41112522.3 (6.1)12321.5 (7.5)0.364
Mental speed
Psif12530.5 (7.6)12628.9 (7.7)0.09812729.8 (7.5)12629.7 (7.9)0.875

NfL and Ng were log-transformed prior to T-tests.

We also examined CSF NfL levels in tertiles. Participants with NfL levels in the highest tertile performed worse on Immediate recall (7.9 versus 8.5, p = 0.021, Cohen’s D = 0.37) and Delayed recall (7.4 versus 7.9, p = 0.041, Cohen’s D = 0.29) compared to those in the lowest tertile. (Supplementary Table 1).

Since the NfL variable had a skewed distribution, we also performed a Mann-Whitney U-test as a sensitivity analysis (Supplementary Table 4), testing differences in cognitive test scores between individuals with NfL above and below the median. The analyses showed differences between the groups in the Immediate recall (p = 0.025) and FAS (p = 0.021) tests. The rank-mean was higher in those with NfL below the median for both tests.

Linear regressions with age, education, sex, and APOE ɛ4 as covariates were also performed to test for an association between cognition and NfL and Ng, but no associations were found (Supplementary Table 3).

Participants with CSF-Ng above the median performed worse in one memory test compared to those below the median (Supra span, 7.5 versus 7.9 p = 0.035, Cohen’s D = 0.28). There were no differences in any other cognitive domains (Table 3).

We then examined CSF Ng in tertiles and detected no differences in cognitive test performance in participants with the highest Ng tertile compared to the lowest Ng tertile. (Supplementary Table 2). We found no association between cognitive test scores and Ng. (Supplementary Table 3), nor did we find a difference between high versus low Ng for any of the cognitive test results in a Mann-Whitney U-test (Supplementary Table 4).

Cognitive performance in 70-year-olds with CDR0 and biomarker evidence of AD pathology stratified by high versus low NfL and Ng concentrations

To compare participants with and without preclinical AD pathology, we stratified the group by high and low levels of NfL and Ng, and divided these groups based on Aβ42 and tau concentrations (the p-tau and t-tau groups merged, since the p-tau group was very small). The group with pathologic tau concentrations may contain participants who also have pathologic amyloid concentrations and vice versa, as this division was necessary to avoid small groups. In the group with high NfL, participants with pathologic tau performed worse on Delayed recall (7.1 versus 8.1, p = 0.003, Cohen’s D = 0.64) than the group without normal tau, and the amyloid pathology group also performed worse than the group without amyloid pathology on Delayed recall (7.3 versus 8.1, p = 0.044, Cohen’s D = 0.45). There were no differences in any other cognitive tests (Table 4).

Table 4

Cognitive performance in 70-year-olds with Clinical Dementia Rating 0 and pathology, stratified by high and low NfL level

NfL above medianNfL below median
AmyloidNo amyloidp1PathologicNormalp2AmyloidNo amyloidp1PathologicNormalp2
pathologypathologytautaupathologypathologytautau
(N = 36)(N = 48)(N = 61)(N = 48)(N = 22)(N = 87)(N = 24)(N = 87)
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
MMSE29.2 (1.0)29.2 (0.8)0.75229.0 (1.2)29.2 (0.8)0.29529.2 (0.8)29.3 (0.9)0.55829.5 (0.6)29.3 (0.9)0.473
Memory
Immediate recall8.0 (1.8)8.2 (1.6)0.5687.7 (1.8)8.2 (1.6)0.1318.5 (1.0)8.5 (1.7)0.8518.7 (1.1)8.5 (1.7)0.393
Delayed recall7.3 (2.0)8.1 (1.5)0.0447.1 (1.6)8.1 (1.5)0.0038.0 (1.5)7.8 (1.8)0.7458.1 (1.7)7.8 (1.8)0.463
Word memory5.2 (1.9)5.5 (1.9)0.5405.4 (1.8)5.5 (1.9)0.8635.9 (1.7)5.9 (1.8)0.9725.8 (1.2)5.9 (1.8)0.894
Supra span (BUSII)7.4 (1.3)7.9 (1.3)0.1337.4 (1.5)7.9 (1.3)0.1028.3 (1.1)7.8 (1.5)0.0597.6 (1.8)7.8 (1.5)0.564
Thurstone’s
Picture memory test21.4 (5.6)23.00 (3.8)0.17122.4 (4.2)23.0 (3.8)0.51022.7 (3.9)23.0 (3.5)0.69522.6 (4.0)23.0 (3.5)0.675
Language
Word fluency25.0 (6.2)25.4 (6.6)0.77324.6 (6.1)25.4 (6.6)0.47226.7 (7.0)25.1 (6.8)0.34023.7 (6.5)25.1 (6.8)0.380
FAS40.3 (11.1)39.9 (11.5)0.86638.8 (13.7)39.9 (11.5)0.66046.7 (15.5)43.0 (14.2)0.31548.5 (14.2)43.0 (14.2)0.127
Executive function
SRB220.19 (3.7)20.3 (4.6)0.88920.2 (3.7)20.3 (4.6)0.82521.0 (4.6)20.7 (4.2)0.77821.3 (4.3)20.7 (4.2)0.513
Digit span backward4.7 (1.0)4.8 (0.9)0.6354.6 (1.1)4.8 (0.9)0.4774.7 (1.1)4.6 (1.3)0.5874.5 (1.1)4.6 (1.3)0.801
Visuospatial
SRB322.0 (6.9)22.4 (6.9)0.77620.5 (7.4)22.4 (6.9)0.17723.3 (6.9)22.1 (6.5)0.48822.2 (7.2)22.1 (6.5)0.959
Mental speed
Psif27.8 (9.4)28.8 (5.8)0.57029.3 (8.8)28.8 (5.8)0.71730.7 (6.7)30.6 (8.2)0.94529.6 (6.5)30.6 (8.2)0.618

Pathologic tau was defined as the t-tau and p-tau groups combined. No amyloid pathology and no tau pathology were defined as A-T-N-.

We also compared participants with pathologic Aβ42 and tau to those with normal concentrations stratified by high/low Ng in the same way. In the high Ng level group, participants with amyloid pathology performed worse on the MMSE (29.0 versus 29.4, p = 0.027, Cohen’s D = 0.46) than participants without amyloid pathology. There were no other differences in any other cognitive tests (Table 5).

Table 5

Cognitive performance in 70-year-olds with Clinical Dementia Rating 0 and pathology, stratified by high and low Ng level

Ng above medianNg below median
AmyloidNo amyloidp1PathologicNormalp2AmyloidNo amyloidp1PathologicNormalp2
pathologypathologytautaupathologypathologytautau
(N = 30)(N = 41)(N = 80)(N = 41)(N = 29)(N = 97)(N = 7)(N = 97)
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
MMSE29.0 (1.0)29.4 (0.7)0.02729.1 (1.1)29.4 (0.7)0.11529.4 (0.8)29.2 (1.0)0.34629.7 (0.5)29.2 (1.0)0.194
Memory
Immediate recall8.0 (1.7)8.4 (1.8)0.3498.0 (1.7)8.4 (1.8)0.1468.3 (1.4)8.4 (1.6)0.8788.3 (1.3)8.4 (1.6)0.904
Delayed recall7.4 (1.9)7.9 (1.7)0.2947.4 (1.7)7.9 (1.7)0.1767.7 (1.8)7.9 (1.7)0.5827.7 (1.6)7.9 (1.7)0.753
Word memory5.4 (1.8)5.9 (1.8)0.3055.6 (1.7)5.9 (1.8)0.3965.5 (1.8)5.7 (1.9)0.5945.1 (1.8)5.7 (1.9)0.468
Supra span (BUSII)7.7 (1.3)7.8 (1.3)0.7977.4 (1.5)7.8 (1.3)0.2378.0 (1.4)7.9 (1.5)0.7187.6 (2.0)7.9 (1.5)0.642
Thurstone’s Picture memory test21.5 (5.7)23.7 (3.0)0.07522.6 (4.2)23.7 (3.0)0.14322.3 (4.3)22.8 (3.8)0.53423.1 (4.3)22.8 (3.8)0.817
Language
Word fluency25.2 (7.2)25.6 (7.2)0.80324.0 (6.2)25.6 (7.2)0.20225.9 (5.9)25.2 (6.6)0.60627.4 (5.0)25.2 (6.6)0.380
FAS42.5 (12.9)43.0 (14.7)0.88741.3 (14.5)43.0 (14.7)0.54942.3 (13.4)42.0 (13.1)0.89540.0 (12.2)42.0 (13.1)0.705
Executive function
SRB220.4 (3.9)20.0 (4.1)0.69220.3 (3.9)20.0 (4.1)0.71520.7 (4.1)20.8 (4.4)0.88622.3 (3.5)20.8 (4.4)0.379
Digit span backward4.7 (1.1)4.7 (1.2)0.9704.6 (1.1)4.7 (1.2)0.4614.6 (1.0)4.6 (1.2)0.9914.4 (1.4)4.6 (1.20)0.746
Visuospatial
SRB322.3 (7.6)22.0 (8.0)0.88721.3 (7.4)22.0 (8.0)0.63422.8 (6.1)22.2 (6.1)0.65219.3 (7.0)22.2 (6.1)0.234
Mental speed
Psif28.5 (9.7)30.2 (7.1)0.40029.6 (8.2)30.2 (7.1)0.70329.8 (7.5)29.8 (7.6)0.99027.4 (7.2)29.8 (7.6)0.431

Pathologic tau was defined as the t-tau and p-tau groups combined. No amyloid pathology was defined as A-T-N-.

We investigated the relationship between pathological tau and Aβ42 concentrations to NfL and Ng concentrations by dividing all participants into tertiles based on their NfL and Ng concentrations. Participants with higher NfL concentrations more often had pathological t-/p-tau concentrations (13%, 33% and 55% in tertile 1, 2, and 3 respectively) (Fig. 1A). The same pattern was seen in the Ng tertiles where 0%, 28%, and 73% in tertile 1, 2, and 3 had pathological t-/p-tau concentrations (Fig. 1B). This pattern was less clear in relation to Aβ42 concentrations where NfL tertile 1, 2, and 3 had 12%, 15%, and 10% of participants with pathological Aβ42 concentrations (Fig. 1A). In the Ng tertiles 20%, 26%, and 23% had pathological Aβ42 concentrations (Fig. 1B).

Fig. 1

Proportions of participants with pathological concentrations of Aβ42 and tau in tertiles of NfL (A) and tertiles of Ng (B).

Proportions of participants with pathological concentrations of Aβ42 and tau in tertiles of NfL (A) and tertiles of Ng (B).

Correction for multiple testing

When performing correction for multiple testing using the Hochberg method, none of the significant p values remained, except the result for delayed recall in Table 4 (p = 0.003).

DISCUSSION

This study examined neurocognitive test performance in cognitively healthy older adults from the general population with and without markers of neurodegeneration (high versus low CSF NfL) and synaptic degeneration (high versus low Ng). Aside from subtle differences in a few cognitive tests, we observed similar test performance in participants with both high and low levels of these biomarkers. However, participants with high NfL performed slightly worse in the memory and the language domain, and those with high Ng performed slightly worse in one memory test, which is in line with our hypothesis, although the effect sizes were small.

The results of our study are in keeping with the results of the cross-sectional population-based study in older adults with normal cognition from the Vanderbilt Memory & Aging Project in Nashville, USA, that found an association between CSF NfL and memory [41], although participants were slightly older than in our study (mean age 72±7 years). Another population-based study from the Mayo Clinic of older adults without dementia investigated associations between CSF and plasma NfL and cognition in five different domains. Although no cross-sectional associations were found between cognition and CSF and plasma NfL, the authors reported longitudinal associations between plasma NfL and worsening cognition, as well as neurodegeneration [42]. As our study is cross-sectional it may be that cognitive decline appears over time in the groups with underlying neurodegeneration as reflected in higher CSF NfL levels, a notion which is supported by the slight decline in two cognitive domains in our study.

Our finding of slightly worse memory in participants with high Ng levels are in line with another cross-sectional study which also found a relationship between Ng and memory in cognitively healthy older adults [43]. In their study, the association between Ng and memory was independent of other AD biomarkers.

Apart from testing in the memory and language domain, we were, however, unable to detect differences between the groups in the majority of the neuropsychological tests, as has been shown before in preclinical AD [44]. Moreover, some findings could be due to multiple testing. The result that participants with NfL levels above median had worse scores in the tests Immediate recall and FAS was also supported by the Mann-Whitney U-test sensitivity analysis. Immediate recall was also significant when dividing NfL into tertiles, where those with the highest NfL performed worse than the reference group (the lowest tertile), indicating that higher levels of NfL indeed reflect more ongoing neurodegeneration. However, in the linear regressions with covariates we could not detect any association between NfL or Ng and any of the cognitive tests, indicating that potential effects of NfL and Ng on test performance are not independent of these covariates.

When we examined participants with underlying preclinical AD pathology in relation to high NfL, we found that having pathologic amyloid and tau concentrations in CSF was related to worse performance in another memory test (Delayed recall). Higher t- or p-tau concentrations is related to ongoing neurodegeneration which is as well reflected in higher NfL levels. We could show that more participants with tau-pathology were in the higher tertiles of NfL whereas more participants with low NfL did not have tau-pathology, indicating that both NfL and tau-pathology relate to the ongoing neurodegeneration. Regarding Ng, we could show that a large portion of participants in the highest tertile of Ng had pathologic tau concentrations while participants in the lower tertiles less often had pathologic tau concentrations, indicating a relationship between neurodegeneration and synaptic pathology. Regarding amyloid pathology, there was a much smaller difference between the highest and lowest tertiles of NfL and Ng. CSF NfL seems to be a marker of unspecific neurodegeneration independent of amyloid pathology, which has been shown before [5].

Since NfL is a marker of neurodegeneration, it seems that a higher level of neurodegeneration is associated with subtly worse cognition in some cognitive tests in otherwise cognitively healthy individuals. High Ng levels were also related to lower performance on a memory test. Synaptic degeneration is an early event in AD. Our results suggest that neuronal damage precedes cognitive decline and that NfL and Ng might be early markers. It is possible that participants with signs of neurodegeneration or synaptic damage may be closer to a conversion to MCI or dementia, although this can only be evaluated in longitudinal data.

Contrary to our hypothesis, there were no differences between individuals with and without preclinical AD pathology, in groups with high NfL level or low NfL level, except that individuals with high NfL levels and preclinical AD (pathologic amyloid and tau concentrations) performed worse in the memory test Delayed recall, and those with Ng and amyloid pathology performed worse in the MMSE. It may be that our participants are so very early in the disease phase (as indicated by the high mean MMSE score) that only subtle differences can be detected.

The results of our study should be interpreted with caution considering the fact that most results did not survive Hochberg correction. However, this method is conservative and may increase the risk of type II errors.

Strengths and limitations

Among the strengths of this study are the comprehensive examinations and the relatively high response rate for LP. Dementia diagnoses were determined by medical and psychiatric experts, and the examinations were performed by experienced research nurses trained by the Principal Investigator. The inter-rater reliability between nurses and psychiatrists diagnosing dementia was high, as previously reported [45].

As for limitations, participants may have perceived the examinations as wearying which might possibly have an effect during cognitive examinations, but our impression is that this has been a minor issue [23].

In observational studies, there could be a potential for selection bias. We have tried to minimize this bias by the systematic selection of participants and the population-based design, which makes our study more representative of the general population than some other studies that rely on convenience samples or volunteers. The nature of the examinations with a long examination time risks leading to a non-response bias with less healthy individuals remaining at home while healthier persons may be more willing to participate. In an effort to prevent this, we offered home visits. Exclusion from LP due to contraindications might also have biased the sample towards healthier persons. Another limitation is that occurrence of spurious significances due to multiple testing cannot be ruled out. Further, it is possible that the study is underpowered in cases where the cohort was divided into small groups.

The relatively young age and health of the participants could be perceived both as a strength and a limitation— given the sparse studies on NfL and Ng in the general population, this sample could provide novel information about cognitive performance in older adults, but nevertheless differences are challenging to detect at such a relatively young age. The cross- sectional design is also a weakness, and a follow-up will be necessary to determine which participants will proceed to develop cognitive decline and AD in the future. Lastly, these findings cannot be extrapolated to other age groups or other nationalities.

Conclusions

This study showed that 70-year-olds with markers of neurodegeneration and synaptic pathology had slightly worse performance in some memory tests, although we could not detect differences in other cognitive tests between individuals with high and low NfL or Ng in most of the tests in the battery. Since being cognitively healthy according to CDR was an inclusion criterion, larger differences between participants could not be expected in this study.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

HZ is a Wallenberg Scholar supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council. (#2018-02532), the European Research Council (#681712 and #101053962), Swedish State Support for Clinical Research (#ALFGBG-71320), the Alzheimer Drug Discovery Foundation (ADDF), USA (#201809-2016862), the AD Strategic Fund and the Alzheimer’s Association (#ADSF-21-831376-C, #ADSF-21-831381-C and #ADSF-21-831377-C), the Olav Thon Foundation, the Erling-Persson Family Foundation, Stiftelsen för Gamla Tjänarinnor, Hjärnfonden, Sweden (#FO2019-0228), the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No 860197 (MIRIADE), the European Union Joint Programme – Neurodegenerative Disease Research (JPND2021-00694), and the UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL (UKDRI-1003).

SK study was financed by grants from the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement (ALFGBG-965923, ALFGBG-81392, ALF GBG-771071). The Alzheimerfonden (AF-842471, AF-737641, AF-939825). The Swedish Research Council (2019-02075), Stiftelsen Psykiatriska Forskningsfonden, Stiftelsen Demensfonden, Stiftelsen Hjalmar Svenssons Forskningsfond, Stiftelsen Wilhelm och Martina Lundgrens vetenskapsfond.

The H70 study was financed by grants from the Swedish state under the agreement between the Swedish government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement (ALF965812, ALF 716681), the Swedish Research Council (2012-5041, 2015-02830, 2019-01096, 2013-8717, 2017-00639, 2019-01096), Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Wellfare (2013-1202, 2018-00471, AGECAP 2013-2300, 2013-2496), Konung Gustaf V:s och Drottning Victorias Frimurarestiftelse, Hjärnfonden (FO2014-0207, FO2016-0214, FO2018-0214, FO2019-0163, FO2020-0235), Alzheimerfonden (AF-554461, AF-647651, AF-743701, AF-844671, AF-930868, AF-940139, AF-940139, AF-968441), Eivind och Elsa K:son Sylvans stiftelse.

Authors’ disclosures available online (https://www.j-alz.com/manuscript-disclosures/22-0452r1).

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

[1] The supplementary material is available in the electronic version of this article: https://dx.doi.org/10.3233/JAD-220452.

REFERENCES

[1] 

Kern S , Zetterberg H , Kern J , Zettergren A , Waern M , Hoglund K , Andreasson U , Wetterberg H , Borjesson-Hanson A , Blennow K , Skoog I ((2018) ) Prevalence of preclinical Alzheimer disease: Comparison of current classification systems. Neurology 90: , e1682–e1691.

[2] 

Rådestig MA , Skoog J , Zetterberg H , Kern J , Zettergren A , Sacuiu S , Waern M , Wetterberg H , Blennow K , Skoog I , Kern S ((2021) ) Cognitive performance and cerebrospinal fluid markers in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Results from the Gothenburg H70 Birth Cohort Studies. J Alzheimers Dis 79: , 225–235.

[3] 

Vos SJ , Xiong C , Visser PJ , Jasielec MS , Hassenstab J , Grant EA , Cairns NJ , Morris JC , Holtzman DM , Fagan AM ((2013) ) Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and its outcome: A longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurol 12: , 957–965.

[4] 

Tarawneh R , D’Angelo G , Crimmins D , Herries E , Griest T , Fagan AM , Zipfel GJ , Ladenson JH , Morris JC , Holtzman DM ((2016) ) Diagnostic and prognostic utility of the synaptic marker neurogranin in Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 73: , 561–571.

[5] 

Kern S , Syrjanen JA , Blennow K , Zetterberg H , Skoog I , Waern M , Hagen CE , van Harten AC , Knopman DS , Jack CR Jr. , Petersen RC , Mielke MM ((2019) ) Association of cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light protein with risk of mild cognitive impairment among individuals without cognitive impairment. JAMA Neurol 76: , 187–193.

[6] 

Zetterberg H , Skillback T , Mattsson N , Trojanowski JQ , Portelius E , Shaw LM , Weiner MW , Blennow K , Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative ((2016) ) Association of cerebrospinal fluid neurofilament light concentration with Alzheimer disease progression. JAMA Neurol 73: , 60–67.

[7] 

Hoffman PN , Cleveland DW , Griffin JW , Landes PW , Cowan NJ , Price DL ((1987) ) Neurofilament gene expression: A major determinant of axonal caliber. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 84: , 3472–3476.

[8] 

Skillback T , Farahmand B , Bartlett JW , Rosen C , Mattsson N , Nagga K , Kilander L , Religa D , Wimo A , Winblad B , Rosengren L , Schott JM , Blennow K , Eriksdotter M , Zetterberg H ((2014) ) CSF neurofilament light differs in neurodegenerative diseases and predicts severity and survival. Neurology 83: , 1945–1953.

[9] 

Norgren N , Rosengren L , Stigbrand T ((2003) ) Elevated neurofilament levels in neurological diseases. Brain Res 987: , 25–31.

[10] 

Khalil M , Teunissen CE , Otto M , Piehl F , Sormani MP , Gattringer T , Barro C , Kappos L , Comabella M , Fazekas F , Petzold A , Blennow K , Zetterberg H , Kuhle J ((2018) ) Neurofilaments as biomarkers in neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurol 14: , 577–589.

[11] 

Mattsson N , Insel PS , Palmqvist S , Portelius E , Zetterberg H , Weiner M , Blennow K , Hansson O ((2016) ) Cerebrospinal fluid tau, neurogranin, and neurofilament light in Alzheimer’s disease. EMBO Mol Med 8: , 1184–1196.

[12] 

Idland AV , Sala-Llonch R , Borza T , Watne LO , Wyller TB , Braekhus A , Zetterberg H , Blennow K , Walhovd KB , Fjell AM ((2017) ) CSF neurofilament light levels predict hippocampal atrophy in cognitively healthy older adults. Neurobiol Aging 49: , 138–144.

[13] 

Camporesi E , Nilsson J , Brinkmalm A , Becker B , Ashton NJ , Blennow K , Zetterberg H ((2020) ) Fluid biomarkers for synaptic dysfunction and loss. Biomark Insights 15: , 1177271920950319.

[14] 

Portelius E , Zetterberg H , Skillback T , Tornqvist U , Andreasson U , Trojanowski JQ , Weiner MW , Shaw LM , Mattsson N , Blennow K , Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative ((2015) ) Cerebrospinal fluid neurogranin: Relation to cognition and neurodegeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 138: , 3373–3385.

[15] 

Becker B , Nazir FH , Brinkmalm G , Camporesi E , Kvartsberg H , Portelius E , Bostrom M , Kalm M , Hoglund K , Olsson M , Zetterberg H , Blennow K ((2018) ) Alzheimer-associated cerebrospinal fluid fragments of neurogranin are generated by Calpain-1 and prolyl endopeptidase. Mol Neurodegener 13: , 47.

[16] 

Wellington H , Paterson RW , Portelius E , Tornqvist U , Magdalinou N , Fox NC , Blennow K , Schott JM , Zetterberg H ((2016) ) Increased CSF neurogranin concentration is specific to Alzheimer disease. Neurology 86: , 829–835.

[17] 

Portelius E , Olsson B , Hoglund K , Cullen NC , Kvartsberg H , Andreasson U , Zetterberg H , Sandelius A , Shaw LM , Lee VMY , Irwin DJ , Grossman M , Weintraub D , Chen-Plotkin A , Wolk DA , McCluskey L , Elman L , McBride J , Toledo JB , Trojanowski JQ , Blennow K ((2018) ) Cerebrospinal fluid neurogranin concentration in neurodegeneration: Relation to clinical phenotypes and neuropathology. Acta Neuropathol 136: , 363–376.

[18] 

Kester MI , Teunissen CE , Crimmins DL , Herries EM , Ladenson JH , Scheltens P , van der Flier WM , Morris JC , Holtzman DM , Fagan AM ((2015) ) Neurogranin as a cerebrospinal fluid biomarker for synaptic loss in symptomatic Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 72: , 1275–1280.

[19] 

Davies CA , Mann DM , Sumpter PQ , Yates PO ((1987) ) A quantitative morphometric analysis of the neuronal and synaptic content of the frontal and temporal cortex in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci 78: , 151–164.

[20] 

Scheff SW , Price DA , Schmitt FA , Mufson EJ ((2006) ) Hippocampal synaptic loss in early Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 27: , 1372–1384.

[21] 

de Wilde MC , Overk CR , Sijben JW , Masliah E ((2016) ) Meta-analysis of synaptic pathology in Alzheimer’s disease reveals selective molecular vesicular machinery vulnerability. Alzheimers Dement 12: , 633–644.

[22] 

Koffie RM , Hyman BT , Spires-Jones TL ((2011) ) Alzheimer’s disease: Synapses gone cold. Mol Neurode-gener 6: , 63.

[23] 

Rydberg Sterner T , Ahlner F , Blennow K , Dahlin-Ivanoff S , Falk H , Havstam Johansson L , Hoff M , Holm M , Horder H , Jacobsson T , Johansson B , Johansson L , Kern J , Kern S , Machado A , Mellqvist Fassberg M , Nilsson J , Ribbe M , Rothenberg E , Ryden L , Sadeghi A , Sacuiu S , Samuelsson J , Sigstrom R , Skoog J , Thorvaldsson V , Waern M , Westman E , Wetterberg H , Zetterberg H , Zetterberg M , Zettergren A , Ostling S , Skoog I ((2019) ) The Gothenburg H70 Birth cohort study 2014-16: Design, methods and study population. Eur J Epidemiol 34: , 191–209.

[24] 

Morris JC ((1993) ) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43: , 2412–2414.

[25] 

Vanmechelen E , Vanderstichele H , Davidsson P , Van Kerschaver E , Van Der Perre B , Sjogren M , Andreasen N , Blennow K ((2000) ) Quantification of tau phosphorylated at threonine 181 in human cerebrospinal fluid: A sandwich ELISA with a synthetic phosphopeptide for standardization. Neurosci Lett 285: , 49–52.

[26] 

Blennow K , Wallin A , Agren H , Spenger C , Siegfried J , Vanmechelen E ((1995) ) Tau protein in cerebrospinal fluid: A biochemical marker for axonal degeneration in Alzheimer disease? Mol Chem Neuropathol 26: , 231–245.

[27] 

Andreasen N , Hesse C , Davidsson P , Minthon L , Wallin A , Winblad B , Vanderstichele H , Vanmechelen E , Blennow K ((1999) ) Cerebrospinal fluid beta-amyloid(1-42) in Alzheimer disease: Differences between early- and late-onset Alzheimer disease and stability during the course of disease. Arch Neurol 56: , 673–680.

[28] 

Jack CR Jr. , Bennett DA , Blennow K , Carrillo MC , Feldman HH , Frisoni GB , Hampel H , Jagust WJ , Johnson KA , Knopman DS , Petersen RC , Scheltens P , Sperling RA , Dubois B ((2016) ) A/T/N: An unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology 87: , 539–547.

[29] 

Hansson O , Zetterberg H , Buchhave P , Londos E , Blennow K , Minthon L ((2006) ) Association between CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer’s disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A follow-up study. Lancet Neurol 5: , 228–234.

[30] 

Hoglund K , Kern S , Zettergren A , Borjesson-Hansson A , Zetterberg H , Skoog I , Blennow K ((2017) ) Preclinical amyloid pathology biomarker positivity: Effects on tau pathology and neurodegeneration, Transl Psychiatry 7: , e995.

[31] 

Palmqvist S , Zetterberg H , Blennow K , Vestberg S , Andreasson U , Brooks DJ , Owenius R , Hägerström D , Wollmer P , Minthon L , Hansson O ((2014) ) Accuracy of brain amyloid detection in clinical practice using cerebrospinal fluid β-amyloid 42: A cross-validation study against amyloid positron emission tomography. JAMA Neurol 71: , 1282–1289.

[32] 

Gaetani L , Hoglund K , Parnetti L , Pujol-Calderon F , Becker B , Eusebi P , Sarchielli P , Calabresi P , Di Filippo M , Zetterberg H , Blennow K ((2018) ) A new enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay for neurofilament light in cerebrospinal fluid: Analytical validation and clinical evaluation. Alzheimers Res Ther 10: , 8.

[33] 

Buschke H , Fuld PA ((1974) ) Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology 24: , 1019–1025.

[34] 

TG TLT ((1949) ) Manual to SRA primary mental abilities, Science Research Associates, Chicago .

[35] 

Dureman I , Kebbon L , Österberg E ((1971) ) Manual till DS-batteriet (Manual for the DS-battery). Psykologiförlaget AB.

[36] 

Thorvaldsson V , Karlsson P , Skoog J , Skoog I , Johansson B ((2017) ) Better cognition in new birth cohorts of 70 year olds, but greater decline thereafter. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 72: , 16–24.

[37] 

Folstein MF , Folstein SE , McHugh PR ((1975) ) “Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12: , 189–198.

[38] 

Berg L ((1988) ) Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR). Psychopharmacol Bull 24: , 637–639.

[39] 

American Psychiatric Association ((1987) ) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-III-R). American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC.

[40] 

American Psychiatric Association ((2013) ) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed American Psychiatric Press, Washington DC.

[41] 

Osborn KE , Khan OA , Kresge HA , Bown CW , Liu D , Moore EE , Gifford KA , Acosta LMY , Bell SP , Hohman TJ , Blennow K , Zetterberg H , Jefferson AL ((2019) ) Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma neurofilament light relate to abnormal cognition. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 11: , 700–709.

[42] 

Mielke MM , Syrjanen JA , Blennow K , Zetterberg H , Vemuri P , Skoog I , Machulda MM , Kremers WK , Knopman DS , Jack C Jr. , Petersen RC , Kern S ((2019) ) Plasma and CSF neurofilament light: Relation to longitudinal neuroimaging and cognitive measures, Neurology 93: , e252–e260.

[43] 

Casaletto KB , Elahi FM , Bettcher BM , Neuhaus J , Bendlin BB , Asthana S , Johnson SC , Yaffe K , Carlsson C , Blennow K , Zetterberg H , Kramer JH ((2017) ) Neurogranin, a synaptic protein, is associated with memory independent of Alzheimer biomarkers. Neurology 89: , 1782–1788.

[44] 

Aizenstein HJ , Nebes RD , Saxton JA , Price JC , Mathis CA , Tsopelas ND , Ziolko SK , James JA , Snitz BE , Houck PR , Bi W , Cohen AD , Lopresti BJ , DeKosky ST , Halligan EM , Klunk WE ((2008) ) Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol 65: , 1509–1517.

[45] 

Wancata J B-HA , Östling S , Sjögren K , Skoog I ((2007) ) Diagnostic criteria influence dementia prevalence. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 15: , 1034–1045.