Searching for just a few words should be enough to get started. If you need to make more complex queries, use the tips below to guide you.
Article type: Research Article
Authors: Drago, Valeriaa; b; c; * | Foster, Paul S.a; d | Ferri, Raffaeleb | Arico, Deborab; c | Lanuzza, Bartolob | Heilman, Kenneth M.a
Affiliations: [a] Department of Neurology, University of Florida, College of Medicine and Neurology Service, Department of Veteran Affairs, Malcom Randal Medical Center, Gainesville, FL, USA | [b] Department of Neurology, Oasi Institute for Research on Mental Retardation and Brain Aging (IRCCS), Troina (EN), Italy | [c] Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy | [d] Department of Psychology, Middle Tennessee State University, Murfreesboro, TN, USA
Correspondence: [*] Corresponding author: Valeria Drago, Department of Neurology, University of Florida, College of Medicine, PO BOX 100236, Gainesville FL 32610-0236, USA. Tel.: +1 352 392 3491; +1 352 494 1681; Fax: +1 352 392 6893; E-mail: [email protected].
Abstract: Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) might have bilateral attentional disorders, such as a reduced spatial attentional window, due to pathological changes in regions important for mediating spatial attention. AD patients may also be highly distractible with the presentation of unilateral novel stimuli or be impaired at disengaging and reallocating their attention with imperative stimuli. This study sought to test these hypotheses by asking AD patients and normal control subjects to bisect 72 horizontal lines of 3 different lengths in three conditions: no lateral stimuli, novel right or left lateral stimuli (‘bottom-up’), and imperative left or right lateral stimuli (‘top-down’). Regarding the bottom-up condition, no group differences emerged, but the AD patients had a greater rightward bias with short lines and a leftward bias with long lines, independent of distracting stimuli. In the top-down condition, when the patients with AD, versus controls, were presented with imperative stimuli on their left side, they demonstrated a greater attentional bias than when presented with right-sided stimuli. Thus AD patients have a reduced spatial attentional window and while they are not highly distracted by novel stimuli, after allocating their attention to left sided stimuli, they have a reduced capacity to spatially re-allocate their attention.
Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, attentional bias, distractibility, line bisection, top-down/bottom-up attention
DOI: 10.3233/JAD-2008-15101
Journal: Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 1-10, 2008
IOS Press, Inc.
6751 Tepper Drive
Clifton, VA 20124
USA
Tel: +1 703 830 6300
Fax: +1 703 830 2300
[email protected]
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to [email protected]
IOS Press
Nieuwe Hemweg 6B
1013 BG Amsterdam
The Netherlands
Tel: +31 20 688 3355
Fax: +31 20 687 0091
[email protected]
For editorial issues, permissions, book requests, submissions and proceedings, contact the Amsterdam office [email protected]
Inspirees International (China Office)
Ciyunsi Beili 207(CapitaLand), Bld 1, 7-901
100025, Beijing
China
Free service line: 400 661 8717
Fax: +86 10 8446 7947
[email protected]
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to [email protected]
如果您在出版方面需要帮助或有任何建, 件至: [email protected]