You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

Apraxia of Speech in the Spontaneous Speech of Nonfluent/Agrammatic Primary Progressive Aphasia

Abstract

Background:

Apraxia of speech (AOS) is a core feature of nonfluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia (naPPA), but its precise characteristics and the prevalence of AOS features in spontaneous speech are debated.

Objective:

To assess the frequency of features of AOS in the spontaneous, connected speech of individuals with naPPA and to evaluate whether these features are associated with an underlying motor disorder such as corticobasal syndrome or progressive supranuclear palsy.

Methods:

We examined features of AOS in 30 patients with naPPA using a picture description task. We compared these patients to 22 individuals with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and 30 healthy controls. Each speech sample was evaluated perceptually for lengthened speech segments and quantitatively for speech sound distortions, pauses between and within words, and articulatory groping. We compared subgroups of naPPA with and without at least two features of AOS to assess the possible contribution of a motor impairment to speech production deficits.

Results:

naPPA patients produced both speech sound distortions and other speech sound errors. Speech segmentation was found in 27/30 (90%) of individuals. Distortions were identified in 8/30 (27%) of individuals, and other speech sound errors occurred in 18/30 (60%) of individuals. Frequent articulatory groping was observed in 6/30 (20%) of individuals. Lengthened segments were observed rarely. There were no differences in the frequencies of AOS features among naPPA subgroups as a function of extrapyramidal disease.

Conclusion:

Features of AOS occur with varying frequency in the spontaneous speech of individuals with naPPA, independently of an underlying motor disorder.

INTRODUCTION

The early modern description of apraxia of speech (AOS) is attributed to Frederic L. Darley et al. [1], who described it as “a disorder of motor speech programming manifested primarily by errors of articulation.” AOS in adults has been observed as a sequela to stroke, head injury, tumor, and neurodegenerative disease. It is one of the core diagnostic criteria for nonfluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia (naPPA) [2], and it may also occur as an independent syndrome, known as primary progressive AOS (PPAOS) [3–7]. There are differing views as to whether it is a disorder of motor programming [8] or motor planning [9] of the speech articulators, but it is agreed that AOS results in inaccurate production of speech sounds due to a motor impairment in the speech production system. This is seen in the presence of effortful, slowed speech, with sound distortions (off-target productions of consonants or vowels), articulatory groping, and prosodic deficits not attributable to dysarthria or cognitive impairment [10–14]. The present report is an examination of the speech of individuals with naPPA with the aim of determining the frequency of occurrence of features of AOS in their spontaneous, connected speech. Studies of AOS frequently include a complete motor examination, with repetition of multisyllabic words and other tasks that challenge the articulatory system. However, in the present study, we examined only speech samples elicited by a picture description task in order to identify and quantify features of AOS that occur in the spontaneous, connected speech of individuals with naPPA. A picture description task is relatively easy for most individuals to perform, as it leaves the speaker free to choose the words s/he can retrieve and produce within the limits of her/his articulatory competence. The description of a scene is similar to the recounting of a first-person narrative in the demands it places on the speaker. This is a speech act that occurs very frequently in everyday life and is therefore valuable in evaluating a person’s ability to function effectively. The assessment of spontaneous, connected speech provides more ecologically valid measures of speech production than do structured speech tasks, which may not reflect the complexities and variability of speech production in real-life situations. It can also provide insights into the neural mechanisms underlying motor speech production. This study extends our understanding of AOS in the speech used in everyday life by persons with naPPA.

naPPA is a progressive disorder of language and speech [15]. The core diagnostic criteria for naPPA, of which at least one must be present, are 1) agrammatic language production and 2) “effortful, halting speech with inconsistent speech sound errors and distortions (apraxia of speech)” [2]. Supporting features include impaired comprehension of syntactically complex sentences, spared single word comprehension, and spared object knowledge.

Impairments in the speech sound production of individuals with naPPA have been extensively documented [15–25]. In previous work, we assessed a semi-structured sample of natural, connected speech [18]. We found frequent phonologic errors that could have been due to an impairment in the linguistic representation of abstract phonological forms [26, 27], but only 31% of patients produced the relatively few phonetic (distortion) errors, that is, speech sounds not present in English, which are more transparently attributed to AOS. These findings may be compared to those of a study by Strand et al. [28], which presented a rating scale for assessing the frequency and severity of features of AOS. These authors examined a cohort of naPPA patients through conversational speech, picture description, word and sentence repetition, and tasks that assess diadochokinesis (DDK). They concluded that most (33/35) patients with naPPA also had AOS. They based their assessments of AOS primarily on the pervasiveness, judged perceptually, of features including sound distortions, syllable segmentation, lengthened vowels and consonants, articulatory groping, and slow speech rate. Since the tasks that assess DDK are specifically designed to bring out motor speech deficits, it would be expected that their use would result in the number of individuals judged to have AOS being greater than when individuals are judged only on the basis of everyday connected speech. Moreover, many of the cases from Strand et al. [28] also had an extrapyramidal movement disorder such as progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) that affects eye movements, gait, and postural stability. There is some uncertainty about whether AOS is an impairment in a general, integrated motor system [29] or whether it is specific to speech [30], as might occur in PPAOS.

To evaluate the possibility that AOS is part of an overall motor programming impairment, we also investigated the contribution of the motor component of AOS by comparing subsets of patients with and without a motor disorder with respect to the presence of features of AOS.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

We studied 30 patients with naPPA, 22 patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal degeneration (bvFTD) as a brain-damaged control group, and 30 healthy seniors (HC). bvFTD was selected as a brain-damaged control group because these patients share some features of impaired speech with naPPA patients. They exhibit slowed speech, and they have been found to make occasional speech sound errors in our previous work [31]. Their speech is similar to that of controls in many respects, but they are similar to naPPA patients in other respects, including number of words produced, degree of grammatical complexity, and report of content. Thus, bvFTD is an intermediate case compared to naPPA and controls.

Participants with naPPA were recruited sequentially between January 2000 and September 2017. Patients were diagnosed by experienced neurologists (MG, DJI) according to published criteria [2]. Exclusionary criteria included other causes of dementia, such as metabolic, endocrine, vascular, structural, nutritional, and infectious etiologies, and primary psychiatric disorders. Also excluded were patients whose speech was so impaired that they were not able to provide a speech sample that was adequate for analysis. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are summarized in Table 1. The groups did not differ in age, sex, or education, and the patient groups did not differ in disease duration. The naPPA and bvFTD groups did not differ in Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score, but both were impaired relative to HC on this measure of global cognitive status.

Table 1

Demographic, clinical, and speech characteristics of participants: Mean (SD)

ParticipantsnaPPAbvFTDHC
N [% M]30 [43]22 [55]30 [43]
Education (y)14.6 (3.1)15.6 (2.5)15.4 (2.3)
Age (y)69.7 (9.1)66.9 (6.1)67.3 (6.8)
Disease duration (y)3.0 (1.6)3.3 (1.8)
MMSE24.2 (5.6)**24.3 (3.2)**29.0 (1.1)
Features of AOS
Sound distortions/100 words1.12 (2.35)0.24 (0.78)
Articulatory groping/100 words0.397 (0.892)0.128 (0.601)
False starts and repairs/100 words8.7 (13.2)*5.7 (6.9)2.7 (2.8)
Segmentation of speech: Silences > 150 ms/100 words36.6 (17.6)**BB12.6 (6.3)*9.3 (4.7)
Duration (ms)894 (402)*1307 (1469)707 (258)
Non-AOS speech and language features Speech output
Sound substitutions/100 words7.83 (11.82)BB0.38 (0.87)
Total speech errors/100 words8.69 (13.05)BB0.62 (1.59)
Total time (s)75 (32)62 (20)*78 (23)
Number of words75 (49)**96 (46)**174 (79)
Speech rate (words per min)61 (28)**BB94 (40)**134 (37)
Articulation rate (syllables/s)2.31 (0.58)**BB3.88 (0.92)4.12 (0.59)
Syllables/word1.126 (0.098)*1.111 (0.074)**1.175 (0.059)
Syllable duration (s)0.462 (0.127)**BB0.274 (0.074)0.248 (0.038)
Number of utterances10.3 (4.9)**12.3 (4.8)**17.0 (6.5)
Mean length of utterance (words)7.0 (2.3)**7.6 (1.9)**10.6 (3.5)
Dysfluencies/ 100 words25.4 (19.6)**B14.1 (11.7)10.0 (6.6)
Structure, grammar, content
Nouns/100 words22.2 (6.5)*BB17.4 (4.7)18.4 (4.4)
Inflected verbs/100 words14.0 (3.9)15.0 (4.2)14.0 (1.9)
Dependent clauses/100 utterances8.4 (12.6)**14.9 (14.1)**47.7 (53.4)
% Well-formed sentences64.9 (34.6)**79.0 (23.2)90.0 (9.3)
Report of contents (max = 9)4.4 (2.2)**5.0 (2.1)**7.2 (1.1)

*Differs from HC, p < 0.05; **differs from HC, p < 0.01; Bdiffers from bvFTD, p < 0.05; BBdiffers from bvFTD, p < 0.01.

Six (20%) of the naPPA patients were also diagnosed with an extrapyramidal disorder, corticobasal syndrome (CBS, N = 4) [32] or PSP (N = 2) [33], and 8 additional patients were noted to have either features suggestive of CBS that did not meet full criteria for this condition or mild features of parkinsonism such as some stooping, with a shortened stride or intermittent lateralized tremor. None of the patients exhibited symptoms of dysarthria, consistent with a review by Duffy et al. [22], which found that forms of dysarthria were not present in a majority of individuals with naPPA. None of the bvFTD patients met criteria for a PPA syndrome or an extrapyramidal disorder, and none of the naPPA patients had changes in behavior and social functioning consistent with bvFTD.

Fifteen of the 30 naPPA participants had a cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) sample available; the samples were collected within a mean of 5.5 months (range 0–20 months) from the time of testing, as described below. Using a validated algorithm to screen for Alzheimer’s disease (AD), we identified 13 of those 15 (87%) cases as having a CSF profile (p-Tau/Aβ< 0.09) consistent with non-AD FTLD underlying pathology [34, 35]. Eleven of the naPPA participants had a brain autopsy, including 7 of those with a CSF sample. Thus a total of 19 patients had biomarker data. Of those, 15 (79%) had FTLD spectrum pathology, and 4 (21%) had probable or confirmed AD. This is consistent with previous reports finding that about 79% of naPPA patients have an underlying tauopathy and about 21% of naPPA cases have AD pathology [36–38]. The distribution of CSF profiles and autopsy-confirmed pathology by clinical phenotype is summarized for the 30 naPPA participants in Table 2.

Table 2

Clinical diagnosis, CSF, and autopsy pathology for 30 naPPA participants1

CaseClinicalCSF-indicatedAutopsy
  nos.diagnosispathologydiagnosis
1–9naPPA
10–11naPPAAD
12–14naPPANon-AD
15naPPANon-ADCBD
16naPPANon-ADPSP
17–18naPPA +
19naPPA +CBD
20–22naPPA +Non-AD
23naPPA+Non-ADGGT
24naPPA +ADAD
25CBSCBD
26CBSADAD
27CBSNon-ADCBD
28CBSNon-ADPick’s disease
29–30PSPNon-AD

1Dash indicates data not available. + indicates medical record note of features suggestive of CBS, mild features of parkinsonism. naPPA, nonfluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia; CBS, corticobasal syndrome; PSP, progressive supranuclear palsy; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; CBD, corticobasal degeneration; GGT, globular glial tauopathy.

All participants completed an informed consent procedure in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania.

Materials and procedures

All participants provided a semi-structured speech sample by describing the Cookie Theft scene from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination [39]. This task is widely used in studies of speech impairment, which provides a degree of reproducibility across studies, and it reliably elicits a speech sample long enough for a speaker to demonstrate his/her competence in producing natural speech. It requires a diverse lexicon and a certain level of skill in managing pronominal reference. At the same time, it elicits a fairly brief speech sample, which means that features of interest can be extracted relatively quickly. In spite of its brevity, as we demonstrated in a previous publication, the speech characteristics elicited by this brief description of a single picture are equivalent to those elicited by a much longer speech task, the narration of the story in a wordless children’s picture book [40].

Participants were asked to describe the Cookie Theft scene in as much detail as they could in about one minute. Prompts were given if the speaker was silent for several seconds, and participants were permitted to continue to speak until they signaled that they were finished. The average (±SD) duration of the speech samples for all participants was 71 (±25) s. The speech samples were analyzed quantitatively for AOS-specific and AOS non-specific speech features, summarized in Table 1, in a manner blinded to other individual patient neurologic characteristics. Features relating specifically to AOS include speech sound distortions and effortful, halting speech, presenting as articulatory groping, including false starts and repairs, and segmented speech, discussed further below. Non-AOS features include sound substitutions, speech rate, mean length of utterance, the grammatical measures of dependent clauses and percentage of utterances that are well-formed sentences, among others, consistent with our previous work [40, 41]. Speech rate was quantified as complete words spoken per minute of recording time, including silences. Articulation rate was quantified as syllables spoken divided by the sum of the periods of continuous speech produced, and syllable duration was calculated as the inverse. The periods of continuous speech were identified and quantified using an automated Speech Activity Detector in order to calculate articulation rate and syllable duration. The speech activity detector is an in-house Gaussian mixture models–hidden Markov models-based speech detector developed at the University of Pennsylvania Linguistic Data Consortium to segment speech samples into segments of speech and silence, allowing the user to derive total speech time with the exclusion of pauses.

Transcription was carried out by a linguist (SA) with extensive experience in phonemic and phonetic transcription of a wide variety of English dialects, in a manner blinded to other individual patient neurologic characteristics. The transcriptions of speech errors were reviewed by a second linguist (GA), who confirmed the transcription of sound distortions, sound substitutions, and uninterpretable words. The earliest speech samples were recorded on cassette tape and subsequently digitized in Praat [42] or recorded on a Marantz PMD 670 digital recorder, while the majority were recorded on smart phones. All recordings were digitized in.wav format (uncompressed) at a sampling rate of 16 KHz and bit depth of 16. The recordings were generally of good quality, and with the aid of high-quality headphones, most of the speech was interpretable and could be transcribed in normal spelling. Some words were readily understandable but were produced with deviations from normal speech, and these were transcribed using phonemic notation, with characters written between slashes. Phonemic notation was also used when an intended word was indeterminate, but the component phonemes were recognizable. Instances where a segment was not identifiable as a phoneme of English were transcribed using phonetic transcription, with characters between square brackets. Speech that was not interpretable was transcribed phonetically or phonemically as needed if ordinary orthography did not adequately indicate the sounds that were spoken. In the entire naPPA corpus, approximately 40/2242 words (1.8%) were not interpretable. Where transcription was not possible, the approximate number of syllables was indicated by sequences of xxx. In the present participant cohort, this occurred only twice, in both cases in the speech of naPPA patients with CBS. Each instance of deviant speech, whether it was a distortion, a phonemic substitution error, an uninterpretable sequence, or a sequence that could not be transcribed, was listed and counted (see Supplementary Material).

Speech errors: Distortions and sound substitutions

The criteria for AOS list “sound distortions” as a core feature [28, 43–46]. This term is used to describe a sound for which the realization “is not quite right” for the target phoneme [43, 47, 48]. In the words of Haley et al. [43], “Segment distortions give the impression that there is something phonetically unusual or incorrect about the sound production.” Distortions contrast with phonemic substitution errors, which are correctly articulated phonemes that are not correctly selected for the corresponding word. Distortions seem necessarily to be due to a motor impairment because they arise when the articulators are not positioned in the right place at the right time to produce the intended speech sound or any acceptable American English phoneme. In the present corpus, for example, a patient said, “her water [oßptr flob]” for “her water overflowed.” The beta character in the transcription is the phonetic character for a “b” with incomplete closure. This non-English sound arises by changing the place of articulation from the place for /v/ (labiodental) to the place for /b/ (bilabial), without changing the manner of articulation. This change of place of articulation is a small step, requiring only advancing the lower lip from proximity to the upper teeth to proximity to the upper lip. In this instance, the following /f/ was also incompletely articulated. Thus, this word contained two phonetic distortions, as well as the phonemic substitution of b for d at the end of the sentence.

The patients produced additional phonemic errors that consisted of accurately articulated phonemes which were not close in articulation to the target, such as “meanmaud” for “meanwhile.” Some phonemic errors resulted in words that were uninterpretable. For example, one patient said, “Bordgin kid getting cookeds out of the colture jan.” It is clear from the picture that cookeds means ‘cookies’ and colture jan means ‘cookie jar’, but the intent of bordgin is unknown. Sound distortions in an unidentifiable word were rare, but they were transcribed phonetically and counted. Otherwise, an unidentifiable word was counted as one phonemic error, on the grounds that it was not possible to count the number of sound substitutions, additions, or deletions in such a word. Sound substitutions in naPPA occurred in 98 out of a total of 2,242 words, of which 39 (1.7%) were not recognizable as real words (such as bordgin), while 59 were clearly interpretable. Since these words consist of identifiable phonemes, rather than distortions, they only affect the count of sound substitutions and total speech errors, and not the count of features of AOS. Among the bvFTD patients, there were 6/2,107 words with phonemic errors, of which 3 (0.14%) were not recognizable as real words.

In what follows, we consider distortions to represent a motor impairment. We will refer to the set of speech errors consisting of distortions and also phonemic substitutions as “total speech errors.” Following Wilson et al. [49], substitution errors were counted as errors even if they were subsequently repaired, but this occurred only once. Incomplete words were not counted as speech errors but rather as false starts. The occurrences of distortions and substitutions were normalized on the total number of complete words spoken by the participant as occurrences per 100 words.

As noted above, major features of AOS other than distortions include the segmentation of speech by brief silences between and within words; lengthened speech segments; and articulatory groping, including false starts and restarts (see below). A feature was judged to count as present in the speech of an individual patient if it occurred with a frequency exceeding an empirically derived threshold. For sound distortions, the threshold was set at 2 standard deviations above the mean of the combined group of bvFTD and HC participants. For the other features, the threshold was set at 2 standard deviations above or below the mean for HC (see below).

Speech segmentation

We measured the frequency and durations of silences between words to address the issue of segmentation of speech in patients. In the present study a threshold of 150 ms was selected empirically, on the basis of the distribution of stop gap durations in the TIMIT database [50], which shows that there are very few (209/20,410, i.e., 1.02%) within-phrase silences greater than 150 ms in duration. To extract the timing information, we used a locally written program to align word boundaries to the speech signal, and the alignment was reviewed and corrected using the signal processing software Praat [42]. Information on downloading the alignment program is given in the Supplementary Material. The durations of all intervals delineated by the alignment procedure were tabulated for each speaker, including both word durations and durations of silences between words, excluding silences between utterances. An utterance is a syntactic unit, defined as an independent clause and all clauses dependent on it [51]. A sentence fragment, missing a subject and/or an inflected verb, was also counted as an utterance if it was not part of any independent clause. Pauses between utterances are not interruptions of the syntactic unit constituted by an utterance and so were not considered to contribute to speech segmentation and were not counted in the assessment of this feature. The number of within-utterance silences was normalized as occurrences per 100 words. Within-word silences were not registered by the alignment algorithm, but they were noted during the process of transcription.

Articulatory groping

The effort to produce a word when the speaker requires multiple attempts to reach the target but does not ultimately succeed in reaching it constitutes articulatory groping, and it may involve either distortions or phonemic substitutions. An instance of articulatory groping is given in Example 1, spoken by an individual with naPPA, age 79, with MMSE = 28, and a disease duration of 3 years. The intended target is given in curly brackets:

(1) I see, cambensen {cabinets}.. camben ness {cabinets}

Another aspect of articulatory groping is the presence of false starts and restarts/repairs [28]. In the present report, we will use the term “repairs” for sequences of words that were repeated or replaced, and we will use the term “false start” to designate a partial word. Example 2, spoken by an individual with naPPA, age 73, with MMSE = 27, and a disease duration of 7 years, illustrates both types. In this example, a false start is shown in italics, and two repaired sequences are underlined.

(2) and one, the man, the kid was, the- the son, was st- uh standing on the, on the stool

The entire phrase that is replaced is counted as one repair, so the sentence in (2) contains 2 repairs and 1 false start. The occurrences of groping and of false starts and repairs were normalized on the total number of complete words spoken by the participant as occurrences per 100 words.

Lengthened speech segments

A speech segment is a single speech sound, either a phoneme or a phone, a vowel, or a consonant. Speech segments were judged perceptually to be lengthened if they were heard as having a longer duration than normal relative to the durations of other segments in the word. Discourse markers, such as and, but, um, and well, were excluded from consideration. The most frequently lengthened segments were the voiceless fricatives /s/ and /f/, as in “ssstool” or “fffinger.” Individual lengthened segments were evaluated perceptually by the first two authors (SA and NN), but quantitative measurement of the duration of each individual segment was beyond the scope of the study.

Each recording was examined in two different ways: 1) For transcription, by the first author, and 2) for perceptual rating of distortion errors, segmentation between and within words, articulatory groping, and lengthened segments independently by the first two authors. The transcription and the perceptual ratings conducted by the first author were performed at different times separated by an interval of at least 2 months.

Statistical considerations

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS v. 27, except for Intraclass Correlation Coefficients, which were calculated in R. Levene’s test of homogeneity of variances indicated that some language variables did not meet the requirement of homogeneity of variances for parametric statistical tests; therefore, we used nonparametric tests to assess the differences within and between subject groups. Comparisons between subject groups were calculated using the Mann-Whitney U statistic. Comparisons of proportions of speakers between groups were calculated using Fisher’s exact test. All reported differences are significant at least at the two-tailed level p < 0.05.

RESULTS

Speech errors: Distortions, substitutions, and total speech errors

Distortions were numerically more frequent in naPPA than in bvFTD, but the difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.095) (Table 1). The frequencies of both substitutions/100 words and of total speech errors/100 words were significantly greater in naPPA (Table 1). The numbers of individuals who exhibited features of AOS are summarized in Table 5. Eight (27%) of the naPPA patients produced distortions, compared to 2 (9%) bvFTD patients, but this difference in prevalence was not statistically significant. Eighteen (60%) of the 30 individuals with naPPA produced interpretable speech errors, either distortions or substitutions, compared to 3 (14%) of 22 bvFTD patients (p < 0.01) and none of the HC participants.

Table 5

Quantitative assessment of AOS-specific and non-AOS specific characteristics by individuals

ParticipantsnaPPAbvFTDHC
N = 30N = 22N = 30
AOS-specific features
Individuals with sound distortions18 (27%)2 (9%)0
Individuals with articulatory groping, not reaching target (1 or 2 occurrences)6 (20%)1 (5%)0
Individuals with false starts and/or repairs23 (77%)17 (77%)24 (80%)
Segmentation: Individuals with≥19 between-word silences per 100 words227 (90%)**BB3 (14%)2 (7%)
Non-AOS-specific features
Individuals with phonemic substitutions18 (60%)BB3 (14%)0
Individuals with speech errors (all error types)18 (60%)BB3 (14%)0
Individuals with WPM < 6317 (57%)**B5 (23%)1 (3%)
Individuals with articulation rate < 0.295325 (83%)**BB4 (18%)1 (3%)
Individuals with syllable duration > 0.324226 (87%)**BB6 (27%)2 (7%)

1Cutoff is 2 SD above the mean for bvFTD and HC combined. 2Cutoff is 2 SD above the HC mean. 3Cutoff is 2 SD below the HC mean. *Differs from HC, p < 0.05; **differs from HC, p < 0.01; Bdiffers from bvFTD, p < 0.05; BBdiffers from bvFTD, p < 0.01.

Segmented speech

The quantified frequency of silences≥150 ms between words per 100 words was significantly greater in naPPA than in bvFTD or HC (p < 0.001 for both comparisons), and bvFTD produced silences between words more frequently than HC, as shown in Table 1. The differences among groups were also reflected in the number of individuals for whom the quantification of such silences revealed a frequency≥2 standard deviations above the mean for HC: 27/30 (90%) naPPA, 3/22 (14%) bvFTD, and 2/30 (7%) of HC participants (Table 5). The silences between words above the calculated threshold (≥19/100 words) were produced by significantly more individuals with naPPA than by those with bvFTD or HC, but there was no significant difference between the number of individuals with bvFTD and HC who produced silences > 150 ms between words above the threshold. The duration of between-word silences was significantly longer in naPPA than in HC.

In contrast to the frequent segmentation between words just described, there were just 9 occurrences of within-word segmentation in naPPA speech, produced by 5 individuals. These few instances were considered to be too infrequent to count as characteristic of the speech of these patients. Six of the cases were compound words: over-flow, over-flows, over-flowed, over-flowing, drive-way, and out-side; one was a phonemic paraphasia for ‘cabinets’: camben-ness; one was a two-syllable word: wo-man; and one was a monosyllable: s-tool. The duration of the within-word silence in these 9 instances ranged from 155 to 557 ms. Within-word segmentation did not occur in the speech of bvFTD or HC participants.

Articulatory groping

When the recordings were reviewed quantitatively for articulatory groping, as illustrated in Example (1) above, 8 instances of groping were found to have been produced by 6 naPPA patients, and one bvFTD patient produced 2 instances. There was no statistically significant difference between naPPA and bvFTD patients in the quantitative assessment of the frequency of false starts and repairs, but there was a significant difference between naPPA and HC, with naPPA producing significantly more false starts and repairs/100 words than HC (Table 1). The proportion of participants who produced false starts and repairs was similar across all three groups (Table 5).

Lengthened segments

Lengthened segments were judged perceptually to be present with greater than moderate frequency (the midpoint on a 5-point scale from “feature not present” to “severe”) compared to controls in 2 of the 30 naPPA speakers. Because they were so infrequent, they were not evaluated quantitatively. A proxy for lengthened segments may be seen in word duration. However, lengthened words are the norm in naPPA, as is reflected by the reduced speech rate of these individuals, and a lengthened word does not necessarily contain individual segments that are lengthened relative to the other segments in the word. A detailed investigation of word duration in these patients and controls is underway in the preparation of a separate paper.

Interrater agreement of judgments of AOS features

Perceptual judgments were made of the essential features of AOS examined for this study: sound distortions, between-word segmentation, within-word segmentation, lengthened segments, and articulatory groping. The judges’ perceptual ratings of features of AOS are summarized in Table 3, and the intraclass correlation coefficients for the ratings are presented in Table 4 [52]. For distortion errors, the correlation was qualitatively interpreted as excellent, according to the guidelines of Koo et al. [52]. For segmented speech (between-word segmentation) and articulatory groping, the correlation was good, and for within-word segmentation and lengthened segments, the correlation was moderate.

Table 3

Perceptual judgments of features of AOS in naPPA by 2 judges, rated on a scale of 0 to 41: Mean (SD)

Rater 1Rater 2
Sound distortions0.90 (1.35)1.10 (1.58)
Segmented speech2.00 (1.34)2.17 (1.42)
Lengthened segments0.23 (0.68)0.90 (0.99)
Segmentation within words0.17 (0.38)0.07 (0.25)
Articulatory groping0.53 (1.04)0.67 (1.32)

10 = feature not present, 1 infrequent, 2 moderate, 3 frequent but not severe, 4 severe.

Table 4

Interrater reliability of judgments of features of AOS in naPPA by two judges

IntraclassQualitative95% ConfidenceF-testp
correlation (A,1)interpretationinterval
Sound distortions0.939Excellent[0.865, 0.971]F(29, 21.5) = 35.9<0.001
Segmented speech0.851Good[0.713, 0.926]F(29, 29.9) = 12.6<0.001
Lengthened segments0.575Moderate[–0.026, 0.829]F(29, 4.25) = 6.890.031
Segmentation within words0.535Moderate[0.231, 0.746]F(29, 28.7) = 3.47<0.001
Articulatory groping0.859Good[0.727, 0.93]F(29, 29.9) = 13.3<0.001

Other features of speech and language: Speech output, structure, grammar, content

Individuals with naPPA differed in predictable ways from those with bvFTD and HC. Their speech rate in words per minute and their articulation rate in syllables per second were less than those of both bvFTD and HC. The speech rate of bvFTD patients was significantly less than that of HC, but bvFTD did not differ from HC in articulation rate. naPPA patients produced fewer words, and their utterances were fewer and shorter. They were more dysfluent, in that they produced more filled pauses, short pauses within utterances, partial words, and extraneous words. They produced fewer dependent clauses, more grammatical errors, and they expressed less of the content of the picture than HC (Table 1).

Effect of extrapyramidal disease

Speech production measures relevant to AOS in the subsets of naPPA patients with and without a clinical diagnosis or medical record notes suggesting CBS or PSP are summarized in Table 6. This table compares patients with 2 or more of the 4 features of AOS that are examined here to those with fewer than 2 features of AOS. Patients were categorized according to phenotype within 15 months of the time of the recording. They were categorized as “No PSP/CBS” if movement disorder symptoms emerged more than 15 months later than the time of the recording. One individual is excluded from the table because the medical records contemporaneous with the recording were unavailable, although there was autopsy-confirmed CBS. Of those patients with 2 or more features of AOS in connected speech, the only statistically significant difference in the measures of speech production between patients with versus without clinical evidence of CBS or PSP was found for the total time of duration of the speech samples: Patients who had no evidence of CBS or PSP took significantly longer to give their picture description than patients with evidence of CBS or PSP. This is a surprising finding which may warrant further investigation, but it does not affect the overall result of the comparison between patients with and without evidence of an extrapyramidal disorder. Of those patients with fewer than 2 features of AOS in connected speech, there were no significant differences in measures of speech production between patients with vs. without clinical evidence of CBS or PSP.

Table 6

Mean frequency (SD) of speech features of naPPA patients with and without at least 2 AOS features and by presence of an extrapyramidal disorder in naPPA1

At least 2≥2 AOSFewer than 2<2 AOS
features of AOSfeatures2features of AOSfeatures2
PSP/CBSNo PSP/CBSpPSP/CBSNo PSP/CBSp
N47612
Sound distortions/ 100 words2.6 (3.5)3.3 (3.1)0.5270 (0)0 (0)1.0
Quantified groping1.1 (1.3)0.89 (1.4)0.7880.22 (0.54)0 (0)0.616
False starts & repairs/100 words16.8 (15.1)19.4 (20.6)1.03.3 (2.4)3.2 (3.8)0.622
Segmentation: Silences≥150 ms/100 words39.6 (17.0)42.7 (16.1)0.78826.4 (10.9)38.1 (21.0)0.213
Silence duration (ms)765 (84)950 (607)0.927834 (330)938 (403)0.616
Sound substitutions/100 words18.4 (10.2)10.6 (10.0)0.3151.9 (2.3)4.8 (14.3)0.616
Total speech errors/100 words20.9 (13.0)13.9 (12.3)0.4123.6 (3.9)4.8 (14.3)0.250
Total time (s)60 (17)105 (29)0.01264 (21)69 (34)0.750
Number of words61 (20)78 (40)0.64875 (56)76 (68)0.682
WPM60.7 (10.8)44.3 (22.1)0.23070.8 (27.5)65.4 (33.2)0.750
Articulation rate (syllables/s)2.20 (0.63)1.94 (0.51)0.5272.56 (0.74)2.37 (0.44)1.0
Number of utterances9.8 (4.3)11.4 (3.3)0.31511.0 (3.2)9.4 (6.7)0.291
MLU (words)6.6 (1.7)6.7 (3.1)0.7886.7 (2.2)7.4 (2.4)0.820
Dysfluencies/100 words40.7 (22.4)41.2 (19.5)0.92715.5 (4.5)17.7 (16.2)0.820
Nouns/100 words19.7 (2.8)24.7 (8.4)0.52719.5 (3.5)23.0 (7.3)0.140
Inflected verbs/100 words14.2 (1.9)13.8 (4.1)0.92715.1 (2.0)13.3 (5.2)0.180
Dependent clauses/100 utterances11.1 (15.7)5.5 (11.3)0.5277.8 (10.0)10.2 (14.8)1.0
% Well-formed sentences42.9 (7.0)45.0 (35.2)0.92776.9 (19.8)75.6 (39.9)0.385
Report of contents (max = 9)4.1 (1.9)3.8 (2.6)0.9275.2 (2.7)4.6 (2.0)0.335
Disease duration (y)2.8 (1.5)3.3 (2.0)0.5272.5 (0.5)3.2 (1.9)0.616

1One individual with autopsy-confirmed CBS is excluded because medical chart notes were unavailable. 2Significance of comparison of individuals with and without evidence of an extrapyramidal disorder (Mann-Whitney U). WPM, speech rate in words per minute; MLU, mean length of utterance in words.

DISCUSSION

In this study we conducted a quantitative assessment of spontaneous, connected speech using a semi-structured speech sample in a cohort of naPPA patients to examine the prevalence of features of AOS that could affect everyday communication in these patients. We investigated four characteristics of AOS: 1) speech sound distortions; 2) segmentation of speech within an utterance, both between words and within words; 3) articulatory groping; and 4) lengthened speech segments. The identification of features of AOS in a person’s speech is valuable because it supports a diagnosis of naPPA. We found that the incidences of the different characteristics indicative of AOS covered a broad range in the spontaneous, connected speech of the patients’ picture descriptions, with speech segmentation being the most frequent (90% of patients) and lengthened segments being the least frequent (7% of patients). We also examined the potential association of extrapyramidal disease with the presence of AOS to assess the hypothesis that a central, praxic deficit gives rise to symptoms of AOS [29]. We found that extrapyramidal disease in naPPA was not related to the presence of AOS features.

Sound distortions

AOS results in alterations of subphonemic detail—that is, sound distortions—in contrast to aphasia, which involves the substitution, addition, or deletion of phonemes and is realized at the level of the mental representation of the linguistic element as a phoneme [43, 53]. Ziegler et al. [54] discuss several possible levels at which apraxia might operate in speech: as a disconnection of phonology (phonological encoding) from motor execution, as a disturbance of learned motor routines (motor planning), or as an impairment of coordination of the temporal and spatial patterning of articulatory movements (motor programming). These authors attribute the uncertainties over the phenomenon of AOS to a “fundamental underspecification of our models describing how stored word form representations are transformed into actual speech movements” (p. S1498).

In the present study, we found that speech errors, including both distortions and sound substitutions, were common, as 18/30 (60%) of the naPPA participants made speech errors. In addition, 3/22 (14%) of bvFTD patients made speech errors, while none of the HC participants produced speech errors in this sample. Of the 18 naPPA patients who made speech errors, only 8 produced distortions at a rate exceeding the threshold of 1.14/100 words calculated for a judgment as a feature of AOS in connected speech. Distortions were observed rarely in bvFTD, in only 2/22 individuals.

The results of this investigation are consistent with those from an earlier study on speech errors in naPPA [18], cited above (p.5). There we found that in a cohort of 16 naPPA patients, 82% of speech errors were sound substitutions, produced by 13/16 patients, and only 18% were distortions, produced by 5/16 patients. These findings were derived from speech samples elicited by asking the participants to narrate the story in a wordless children’s picture book, Frog, Where Are You, which yielded speech samples averaging 346 words for naPPA patients. Although this earlier study included fewer participants, the large quantity of speech provided us with a basis for confirming this level of speech errors in the spontaneous, connected speech of naPPA. The results of prevalence and speech error frequency are similar to those of the present study, confirming the use of a short picture description as a speech sample for studying features of AOS in spontaneous, connected speech.

In another investigation, Knibb et al. [56] characterized quantitatively the conversational speech in a group of naPPA patients. Seven of the 15 patients (47%) had previously been rated as having dysarthria, AOS, or both. The authors did not distinguish types of speech errors, stating that it is not possible to say for an individual speech error whether a distortion is due to dysarthria or apraxia and whether a substitution is a phonemic error or an articulatory error. They found that the patients had a speech error rate of approximately 4.6/100 words, compared to an average for controls of 0.4/100 words. These figures are comparable to those found in the present study, with an overall speech error rate of 5.50/100 words in naPPA, 0.49/100 words in bvFTD, and 0/100 words in HC. Rohrer et al. [57] also examined conversational speech in a cohort of 24 patients with naPPA. Connected speech was elicited by a conversation about the participant’s last holiday celebration and a description of the Cookie Theft scene. They reported a frequency of AOS of 14/24 (58%). As in the report of Knibb et al. [56], these authors included all types of speech errors in their assessment of AOS and found a frequency of speech errors of about 6.2/100 words. Again, these frequencies are comparable to our results.

The approach of these two studies, in which all types of speech errors were considered in the assessment of AOS, accords with the view presented by Laganaro [26], who provides evidence for interaction between the phonological and phonetic encoding levels, entailing simultaneous activation of target and nontarget phonemes, triggered by the activation of competing lexical forms. The competition between phonemes may produce either phonetic or phonological errors. The prospect of a phonological impairment giving rise to phonetic errors blurs the line between a phonological impairment and a motor programming impairment as a source of the speech errors that are observed in AOS. Such considerations may account in part for the difference between the quantified assessments evaluated in the present study and clinical impressions in some published reports of the speech of naPPA patients [49, 58, 59], as we discuss below.

Several studies have utilized the Motor Speech Evaluation (MSE) [14] to determine the frequency of AOS. This evaluation instrument includes sections testing DDK, word repetition, and reading of the Grandfather Passage. A number of these studies have reported frequencies of AOS of, for example, 79% [49], 82% [60], 89% [58], and 100% [25]. Tasks in the MSE battery such as alternating motion rate (“papapa”) and sequential motion rate (“pataka”) can elicit speech errors, even in controls when made adequately difficult, and we expect that speech errors elicited by DDK tasks are likely to overestimate the speech errors that occur in spontaneous connected speech. In contrast to our findings, for example, Wilson et al. [49] assessed AOS on the basis of the MSE, although their study focused on the connected speech of the participants. They report that every naPPA participant in their study produced sound distortions, based on clinical impressions of connected speech elicited by a picture description task, but that phonological errors (i.e., sound substitutions) were produced by only a subset of patients. They arrived at a frequency of distortions in naPPA of 12.6/100 words and a frequency of phonological errors of 1.4/100 words. The corresponding frequencies in bvFTD were 0.4/100 words for distortions and 0.0/100 words for substitutions; for HC, the corresponding frequencies were 0.1/100 words for distortions and 0.1/100 words for substitutions. Their findings for bvFTD and HC are similar to those of the present study, but their results for naPPA give a higher frequency of distortions and a lower frequency of substitutions than the present study. Part of the difference in findings may be attributable to differing definitions of what constitutes a distortion. For example, if “tepping” for “tipping” is heard as a distortion, that would account for much of the discrepancy between our results and those of other authors. In the present study, we judge “tepping” to be a phonemic substitution, not the result of a motor impairment yielding a distortion, since “tepping” is an allowable sequence, which obeys the rules of English word formation and is composed of sounds from the inventory of English speech sounds. Additional work is needed to develop a common set of criteria for speech distortions and phonologic errors across laboratories studying speech. Agreement is also needed on the context of speech–spontaneous or elicited—in order to elucidate the frequency and nature of speech errors in PPA patients.

Segmented speech

The segmentation of speech by silent pauses (>150 ms) between words is a prominent feature in naPPA and contributes to the slow overall rate of speech of these individuals. Speech segmentation is cited as a feature of AOS in qualitative terms, both within words and between words within sentences [28, 59, 61–63]. In the present study, we chose a duration threshold value for silences based on the occurrence of silences in a large corpus of read speech. We found that the frequency of these silences in our sample of naPPA patients was four times the frequency observed in HC and three times the frequency found in bvFTD. Indeed, this feature was almost universal in the naPPA group, which differed significantly from its prevalence in bvFTD and HC. It should however be noted that, while silences between words may be partly attributable to AOS, there are potentially other contributing factors. For example, the non-fluent speech of naPPA patients resulting from prolonged pauses has been related to difficulty in building the grammatical structure of a sentence [17].

In contrast to segmentation between words, we found that segmentation within words was rare, and 6 of the 9 identified instances occurred between syllables that are also independent morphemes, such as over-flow and drive-way, where segmentation between syllables is similar to segmentation between words. Segmentation within words does not appear to be a significant characteristic of speech in naPPA, at least not in the production of spontaneous, connected speech. This may reflect the fact that spontaneous speech allows the speaker to choose words that s/he is able to pronounce with minimal difficulty.

Articulatory groping, false starts, and repairs

Articulatory groping without reaching the intended target is listed as a characteristic feature of AOS. It did not occur with high frequency in naPPA, but it co-occurred with speech segmentation in every case where it was present. Thus, articulatory groping may be strongly suggestive of AOS when it is detected, despite its relatively infrequent occurrence. A more detailed examination of articulatory groping in naPPA, taking into account such factors as phrase length and adjacent pauses, may further illuminate this feature. Unlike groping, false starts and repairs were observed in all 3 participant groups. In naPPA, they were produced with high frequency (>2 SD above the HC mean) by 5 patients, all of whom exhibited speech segmentation, and 2 of whom also produced sound distortions above the threshold frequency. However, the percentages of individuals who produced false starts and repairs were the same in naPPA and bvFTD and were almost identical to the percentage in HC (Table 5).

Perceptual judgments of AOS features

Wambaugh et al. [64] investigated the interrater reliability of judgments of features of AOS in 28 post-stroke patients, using the features of Strand et al. [65]. They found good agreement between two raters for phonetic errors, segmented speech, and within-word segmentation, but poor agreement for judgments both of groping and of false starts/restarts. They propose that judgments of those two categories may have been differently assigned by the two raters. In the present study, we found good agreement between the two raters on judgments of groping, as shown in Table 4. False starts/repairs were only assessed quantitatively, based on the transcripts of the recordings, and are shown in Tables 1, 4, and 5. We also found excellent agreement on the perceptual judgments of sound distortions, and we found good agreement on judgments of speech segmentation, the most prominent prosodic feature of AOS. These results support our confidence in the validity of the quantitative measurements. We found only moderate agreement between the raters on the perceptual judgments of lengthened segments and within-word silences. This limited level of agreement may be due to the fact that these two features were found to be infrequent and therefore relatively less reliable in contributing to the impression of impaired speech in naPPA. In addition, these two features are difficult to hear and may be missed in a perceptual assessment; it would be valuable to evaluate them instrumentally for quantitative assessment.

Extrapyramidal disease

naPPA can be seen in the setting of CBS or PSP. It has been suggested that AOS may be an early sign of a motor disorder, as several case studies have found [66–68]. Another case report describes the progression of naPPA with AOS to CBS [69]. In a study of a cohort of patients with a diagnosis of progressive aphasia or AOS and with autopsy confirmation, 9 out of 10 cases that were determined to have had AOS had a pathological diagnosis of PSP or CBD [24]. A review of cases [23] provides further support for the suggestion that a presentation of naPPA with AOS may predict the pathology of CBD or PSP, and a longitudinal study of AOS found that patients may progress over time to show clinical features consistent with PSP or CBS [5]. Another study from our center was designed to test the hypothesis that patients with PSP and CBS exhibit speech impairments found in naPPA. The analysis of speech samples elicited by description of the Cookie Theft scene found that out of 82 cases (selected for the availability of clinical charts) of PSP/CBS, naPPA, or naPPA with co-occurring PSP/CBS, only 5 (6.1%) had documented features that were consistent with AOS. This included 2 patients with PSP/CBS, 1 with naPPA, and 2 with PSP/CBS + naPPA [70].

In the present study, we sought to assess whether the presence of features of AOS in naPPA would be more frequent in the spontaneous speech of patients with extrapyramidal disease than in those without. Contrary to our expectation, our results did not suggest greater frequency of AOS coincident with extrapyramidal disease. Of the 30 naPPA patients, 14 had evidence of a movement disorder during life, and 10 of these individuals had recorded the Cookie Theft picture description within 15 months of exhibiting extrapyramidal symptoms. Comparisons of the groups with and without clinically assessed symptoms of a movement disorder and with and without biomarker or autopsy evidence of CBD or PSP found only one minor, irrelevant difference among the groups in the incidence of features of speech production. These results fail to support the proposal that AOS is part of an overall motor impairment specifically related to extrapyramidal disease. The relationship between AOS and dysarthric features was not evaluated in this study, because the participants with naPPA did not show evidence of dysarthria.

Our study has certain limitations. While we examined a relatively large number of patients with the rare condition of naPPA, several aspects of our study would have benefitted from a larger sample. In particular, the number of cases for which we were able to estimate from biomarkers or know from autopsy the pathology in our cohort was relatively small. We contrasted naPPA with a neurodegenerative control group and found that features of AOS were more frequent in naPPA, but future work should contrast AOS in naPPA with relevant speech features in other variants of PPA. We focused on spontaneous, connected speech in this study, and the value and reliability of this approach can be determined only in the context of a comparative examination with elicited forms such as those in DDK speech.

One factor that may contribute to the frequency of features of AOS in our data could be that a picture description task is less demanding of the speaker than some other tasks. For example, an interview about familiar topics requires more choices about what elements to report and what words to use than does a picture description, where the stimulus is present. The speech samples elicited in this study were rather short, with an average length of 75 words for the naPPA patients. While this may be seen as a limitation of the study, it appeared that the frequency of both speech errors and speech segmentation was great enough so that there was time for both impairments to be manifested, suggesting that the duration of the speech sample was adequate for the speakers to demonstrate their competence in speech production. However, a longer speech sample would increase our confidence in these findings.

Conclusions

With these caveats in mind, this quantitative examination of the speech of patients with naPPA in describing a picture found that at least one feature of AOS was expressed by 90% of the patients, and two or more features were observed in 37% of the patients. We found that some naPPA patients may have an impairment of their ability to assemble the motor program for the accurate production of speech segments, resulting in sound distortions. In addition, speech segmentation is frequent in naPPA and contributes to the presence of AOS in these patients. Articulatory groping and false starts and repairs provide further evidence of AOS in naPPA. We did not find an association of an underlying motor disorder such as CBS or PSP with a motor impairment in speech production. However, our findings do not allow us to specify the precise location(s) in the process of speech production where a breakdown occurs. Further examination of the impairment in naPPA speech would profitably be directed at addressing the “fundamental underspecification” [54] in our understanding of how word forms are selected and assembled to be transformed into speech.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Galit Agmon, PhD, a linguist and a colleague in the Penn FTD Center, for reviewing the recordings to judge sound distortions, phonemic errors, and nonwords.

FUNDING

This work was supported in part by NIH (AG054519 (MG), AG052943 (MG), AG066597 (MG, DJI, EBL), AG072979 (LS, EBL, JQT), AG062418 (JQT, DJI, EBL, MG), NS109260 (DJI), DC013063 (SS)), Department of Defense W81XWH-20-1-0531 (MG, NN, SA, SS), Alzheimer’s Association AACSF-18-567131 (NN), The University of Pennsylvania Institute on Aging (DJI), and the Samuel Newhouse Foundation (MG).

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

DATA AVAILABILITY

The data used in this study will be made available upon reasonable request from qualified investigators for research purposes upon approval by the University of Pennsylvania.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

[1] The supplementary material is available in the electronic version of this article: https://dx.doi.org/10.3233/ADR-220089.

REFERENCES

[1] 

Darley FL , Aronson AE , Brown JR (1975) Motor Speech Disorders, Saunders, Philadelphia.

[2] 

Gorno-Tempini ML , Hillis AE , Weintraub S , Kertesz A , Mendez M , Cappa SF , Ogar JM , Rohrer JD , Black S , Boeve B , Manes F , Dronkers NF , Vandenberghe R , Rascovsky K , Patterson K , Miller BL , Knopman DS , Hodges JR , Mesulam M-M , Grossman M ((2011) ) Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology 76: , 1006–1014.

[3] 

Josephs KA , Duffy JR , Strand EA , Machulda MM , Senjem ML , Master AV , Lowe VJ , Jack CR Jr., Whitwell JL ((2012) ) Characterizing a neurodegenerative syndrome: Primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain 135: , 1522–1536.

[4] 

Duffy JR , Strand EA , Clark H , Machulda M , Whitwell JL , Josephs KA ((2015) ) Primary progressive apraxia of speech: Clinical features and acoustic and neurologic correlates. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 24: , 88–100.

[5] 

Josephs KA , Duffy JR , Strand EA , Machulda MM , Senjem ML , Gunter JL , Schwarz CG , Reid RI , Spychalla AJ , Lowe VJ , Jack CR Jr., Whitwell JL ((2014) ) The evolution of primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain 137: , 2783–2795.

[6] 

Utianski RL , Duffy JR , Clark HM , Strand EA , Boland SM , Machulda MM , Whitwell JL , Josephs KA ((2018) ) Clinical progression in four cases of primary progressive apraxia of speech. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 27: , 1303–1318.

[7] 

Botha H , Utianski RL , Whitwell JL , Duffy JR , Clark HM , Strand EA , Machulda MM , Tosakulwong N , Knopman DS , Petersen RC , Jack CR Jr., Josephs KA , Jones DT ((2018) ) Disrupted functional connectivity in primary progressive apraxia of speech. Neuroimage Clin 18: , 617–629.

[8] 

Miller HE , Guenther FH ((2021) ) Modelling speech motor programming and apraxia of speech in the DIVA/GODIVA neurocomputational framework. Aphasiology 35: , 424–441.

[9] 

Van Der Merwe A ((2021) ) New perspectives on speech motor planning and programming in the context of the four-level model and its implications for understanding the pathophysiology underlying apraxia of speech and other motor speech disorders. Aphasiology 35: , 397–423.

[10] 

McNeil MR , Robin DA , Schmidt RA (1997) Apraxia of speech: Definition, differentiation, and treatment. In Clinical Management of Sensorimotor Speech Disorders, McNeil MR, ed. Thieme, New York, pp. 311-344.

[11] 

Croot K ((2002) ) Diagnosis of AOS: Definition and criteria. Semin Speech Lang 23: , 267–280.

[12] 

Duffy JR , Josephs KA ((2012) ) The diagnosis and understanding of apraxia of speech: Why including neurodegenerative etiologies may be important. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55: , S1518–1522.

[13] 

Ogar JM , Slama H , Dronkers N , Amici S , Gorno-Tempini ML ((2005) ) Apraxia of speech: An overview. Neurocase 11: , 427–432.

[14] 

Wertz RT , LaPointe LL , Rosenbek JC (1984) Apraxia of speech: The disorders and its management, Grune and Stratton, New York.

[15] 

Grossman M ((2012) ) The non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Lancet Neurol 11: , 545–555.

[16] 

Grossman M ((2018) ) Linguistic aspects of primary progressive aphasia. Annu Rev Linguist 4: , 377–403.

[17] 

Gunawardena D , Ash S , McMillan C , Avants B , Gee J , Grossman M ((2010) ) Why are patients with progressive nonfluent aphasia nonfluent? Neurology 75: , 588–594.

[18] 

Ash S , McMillan C , Gunawardena D , Avants B , Morgan B , Khan A , Moore P , Gee J , Grossman M ((2010) ) Speech errors in progressive non-fluent aphasia. Brain Lang 113: , 13–20.

[19] 

Duffy JR , Hanley H , Utianski R , Clark H , Strand E , Josephs KA , Whitwell JL ((2017) ) Temporal acoustic measures distinguish primary progressive apraxia of speech from primary progressive aphasia. Brain Lang 168: , 84–94.

[20] 

Rohrer JD , Rossor MN , Warren JD ((2010) ) Apraxia in progressive nonfluent aphasia. J Neurol 257: , 569–574.

[21] 

Jung Y , Duffy JR , Josephs KA ((2013) ) Primary progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech. Semin Neurol 33: , 342–347.

[22] 

Duffy JR , Strand EA , Josephs KA ((2014) ) Motor speech disorders associated with primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology 28: , 1004–1017.

[23] 

Josephs KA , Duffy JR ((2008) ) Apraxia of speech and nonfluent aphasia: A new clinical marker for corticobasal degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy. Curr Opin Neurol 21: , 688–692.

[24] 

Josephs KA , Duffy JR , Strand EA , Whitwell JL , Layton KF , Parisi JE , Hauser MF , Witte RJ , Boeve BF , Knopman DS , Dickson DW , Jack CR Jr., Petersen RC ((2006) ) Clinicopathological and imaging correlates of progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech. Brain 129: , 1385–1398.

[25] 

Ogar JM , Dronkers NF , Brambati SM , Miller BL , Gorno-Tempini ML ((2007) ) Progressive nonfluent aphasia and its characteristic motor speech deficits. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 21: , S23–30.

[26] 

Laganaro M ((2012) ) Patterns of impairments in AOS and mechanisms of interaction between phonological and phonetic encoding. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55: , S1535–1543.

[27] 

Levelt WJ , Roelofs A , Meyer AS ((1999) ) A theory of lexical access in speech production.1-38; discussion. Behav Brain Sci 22: , 38–75.

[28] 

Strand EA , Duffy JR , Clark HM , Josephs K ((2014) ) The Apraxia of Speech Rating Scale: A tool for diagnosis and description of apraxia of speech. J Commun Disord 51: , 43–50.

[29] 

Ballard KJ , Robin DA , Folkins JW ((2003) ) An integrative model of speech motor control: A response to Ziegler. Aphasiology 17: , 37–48.

[30] 

Maas E , Robin DA , Wright DL , Ballard KJ ((2008) ) Motor programming in apraxia of speech. Brain Lang 106: , 107–118.

[31] 

Ash S , Moore P , Vesely L , Gunawardena D , McMillan C , Anderson C , Avants B , Grossman M ((2009) ) Non-fluent speech in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J Neurolinguistics 22: , 370–383.

[32] 

Armstrong MJ , Litvan I , Lang AE , Bak TH , Bhatia KP , Borroni B , Boxer AL , Dickson DW , Grossman M , Hallett M , Josephs KA , Kertesz A , Lee SE , Miller BL , Reich SG , Riley DE , Tolosa E , Troster AI , Vidailhet M , Weiner WJ ((2013) ) Criteria for the diagnosis of corticobasal degeneration. Neurology 80: , 496–503.

[33] 

Hoglinger GU , Respondek G , Stamelou M , Kurz C , Josephs KA , Lang AE , Mollenhauer B , Muller U , Nilsson C , Whitwell JL , Arzberger T , Englund E , Gelpi E , Giese A , Irwin DJ , Meissner WG , Pantelyat A , Rajput A , van Swieten JC , Troakes C , Antonini A , Bhatia KP , Bordelon Y , Compta Y , Corvol JC , Colosimo C , Dickson DW , Dodel R , Ferguson L , Grossman M , Kassubek J , Krismer F , Levin J , Lorenzl S , Morris HR , Nestor P , Oertel WH , Poewe W , Rabinovici G , Rowe JB , Schellenberg GD , Seppi K , van Eimeren T , Wenning GK , Boxer AL , Golbe LI , Litvan I ((2017) ) Clinicaldiagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy: The movement disorder society criteria. Mov Disord 32: , 853–864.

[34] 

Irwin DJ , Lleo A , Xie SX , McMillan CT , Wolk DA , Lee EB , Van Deerlin VM , Shaw LM , Trojanowski JQ , Grossman M ((2017) ) Ante mortem cerebrospinal fluid tau levels correlate with postmortem tau pathology in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Ann Neurol 82: , 247–258.

[35] 

Lleo A , Irwin DJ , Illan-Gala I , McMillan CT , Wolk DA , Lee EB , Van Deerlin VM , Shaw LM , Trojanowski JQ , Grossman M ((2018) ) A 2-step cerebrospinal algorithm for the selection of frontotemporal lobar degeneration subtypes. JAMA Neurol 75: , 738–745.

[36] 

Bergeron D , Gorno-Tempini ML , Rabinovici GD , Santos-Santos MA , Seeley W , Miller BL , Pijnenburg Y , Keulen MA , Groot C , van Berckel BNM , van der Flier WM , Scheltens P , Rohrer JD , Warren JD , Schott JM , Fox NC , Sanchez-Valle R , Grau-Rivera O , Gelpi E , Seelaar H , Papma JM , van Swieten JC , Hodges JR , Leyton CE , Piguet O , Rogalski EJ , Mesulam MM , Koric L , Nora K , Pariente J , Dickerson B , Mackenzie IR , Hsiung GR , Belliard S , Irwin DJ , Wolk DA , Grossman M , Jones M , Harris J , Mann D , Snowden JS , Chrem-Mendez P , Calandri IL , Amengual AA , Miguet-Alfonsi C , Magnin E , Magnani G , Santangelo R , Deramecourt V , Pasquier F , Mattsson N , Nilsson C , Hansson O , Keith J , Masellis M , Black SE , Matias-Guiu JA , Cabrera-Martin MN , Paquet C , Dumurgier J , Teichmann M , Sarazin M , Bottlaender M , Dubois B , Rowe CC , Villemagne VL , Vandenberghe R , Granadillo E , Teng E , Mendez M , Meyer PT , Frings L , Lleo A , Blesa R , Fortea J , Seo SW , Diehl-Schmid J , Grimmer T , Frederiksen KS , Sanchez-Juan P , Chetelat G , Jansen W , Bouchard RW , Laforce RJ , Visser PJ , Ossenkoppele R ((2018) ) Prevalence of amyloid-beta pathology in distinct variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 84: , 729–740.

[37] 

Spinelli EG , Mandelli ML , Miller ZA , Santos-Santos MA , Wilson SM , Agosta F , Grinberg LT , Huang EJ , Trojanowski JQ , Meyer M , Henry ML , Comi G , Rabinovici G , Rosen HJ , Filippi M , Miller BL , Seeley WW , Gorno-Tempini ML ((2017) ) Typical and atypical pathology in primary progressive aphasia variants. Ann Neurol 81: , 430–443.

[38] 

Mesulam M-M , Weintraub S , Rogalski EJ , Wieneke C , Geula C , Bigio EH ((2014) ) Asymmetry and heterogeneity of Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal pathology in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 137: , 1176–1192.

[39] 

Goodglass H , Kaplan E (1972) Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination.

[40] 

Ash S , Evans E , O’Shea J , Powers J , Boller A , Weinberg D , Haley J , McMillan C , Irwin DJ , Rascovsky K , Grossman M ((2013) ) Differentiating primary progressive aphasias in a brief sample of connected speech. Neurology 81: , 329–336.

[41] 

Ash S , Jester C , York C , Kofman OL , Langey R , Halpin A , Firn K , Dominguez Perez S , Chahine L , Spindler M , Dahodwala N , Irwin DJ , McMillan C , Weintraub D , Grossman M ((2017) ) Longitudinal decline in speech production in Parkinson’s disease spectrum disorders. Brain Lang 171: , 42–51.

[42] 

Boersma P , Weenink D , (1992-2014) Praat, v. 5.3.76. Institute of Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam.

[43] 

Haley KL , Jacks A , Richardson JD , Wambaugh JL ((2017) ) Perceptually salient sound distortions and apraxia of speech: A performance continuum. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 26: , 631–640.

[44] 

Ballard KJ , Wambaugh JL , Duffy JR , Layfield C , Maas E , Mauszycki S , McNeil MR ((2015) ) Treatment for acquired apraxia of speech: A systematic review of intervention research between 2004 and 2012. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 24: , 316–337.

[45] 

Duffy JR (2013) Motor Speech Disorders: Substrates, Differential Diagnosis, and Management (3rd ed.), Elsevier Mosby, St. Louis, MO.

[46] 

McNeil M , Robin D , Schmidt R (2009) Apraxia of speech: Definition, differentiation, and treatment. In Clinical Management of Sensorimotor Speech Disorders (2nd edition), McNeil M, ed. Thieme, New York, NY.

[47] 

Odell K , McNeil MR , Rosenbek JC , Hunter L ((1990) ) Perceptual characteristics of consonant production by apraxic speakers. J Speech Hear Disord 55: , 345–359.

[48] 

Galluzzi C , Bureca I , Guariglia C , Romani C ((2015) ) Phonological simplifications, apraxia of speech and the interaction between phonological and phonetic processing. Neuropsychologia 71: , 64–83.

[49] 

Wilson SM , Henry ML , Besbris M , Ogar JM , Dronkers NF , Jarrold W , Miller BL , Gorno-Tempini ML ((2010) ) Connected speech production in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Brain 133: , 2069–2088.

[50] 

Garofolo JS , Lamel LF , Fisher WM , Fiscus JG , Pallett DS , Dahlgren NL , Zue V (1993) TIMIT Acoustic-Phonetic Continuous Speech Corpus. Linguistic Data Consortium, Philadelphia, PA.

[51] 

Hunt KW (1965) Grammatical structures written at three grade levels, National Council of Teachers of English, Champaign, IL.

[52] 

Koo TK , Li MY ((2016) ) A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropr Med 15: , 155–163.

[53] 

Haley KL , Jacks A , de Riesthal M , Abou-Khalil R , Roth HL ((2012) ) Toward a quantitative basis for assessment and diagnosis of apraxia of speech. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55: , S1502–1517.

[54] 

Ziegler W , Aichert I , Staiger A ((2012) ) Apraxia of speech: Concepts and controversies. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55: , S1485–1501.

[55] 

Mayer M (1969) Frog, Where Are You?, Penguin Books, New York.

[56] 

Knibb JA , Woollams AM , Hodges JR , Patterson K ((2009) ) Making sense of progressive non-fluent aphasia: An analysisof conversational speech. Brain 132: (Pt 10), 2734–2746.

[57] 

Rohrer JD , Rossor MN , Warren JD ((2010) ) Syndromes of nonfluent primary progressive aphasia: A clinical and neurolinguistic analysis. Neurology 75: , 603–610.

[58] 

Croot K , Ballard K , Leyton CE , Hodges JR ((2012) ) Apraxia of speech and phonological errors in the diagnosis of nonfluent/agrammatic and logopenic variants of primary progressive aphasia.S. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55: , 1562–1572.

[59] 

Leyton CE , Villemagne VL , Savage S , Pike KE , Ballard KJ , Piguet O , Burrell JR , Rowe CC , Hodges JR ((2011) ) Subtypes of progressive aphasia: Application of the International Consensus Criteria and validation using beta-amyloid imaging. Brain 134: , 3030–3043.

[60] 

Gorno-Tempini ML , Dronkers NF , Rankin KP , Ogar JM , Phengrasamy L , Rosen HJ , Johnson JK , Weiner MW , Miller BL ((2004) ) Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 55: , 335–346.

[61] 

Josephs KA , Duffy JR , Strand EA , Machulda MM , Senjem ML , Lowe VJ , Jack CR Jr., Whitwell JL ((2013) ) Syndromes dominated by apraxia of speech show distinct characteristics from agrammatic PPA. Neurology 81: , 337–345.

[62] 

Duffy JR ((2006) ) Apraxia of speech in degenerative neurologic disease. Aphasiology 20: , 511–527.

[63] 

Whitwell JL , Weigand SD , Duffy JR , Clark HM , Strand EA , Machulda MM , Spychalla AJ , Senjem ML , Jack CR Jr. , Josephs KA ((2017) ) Predicting clinical decline in progressive agrammatic aphasia and apraxia of speech. Neurology 89: , 2271–2279.

[64] 

Wambaugh JL , Bailey DJ , Mauszycki SC , Bunker LD ((2019) ) Interrater reliability and concurrent validity for the Apraxia of Speech Rating Scale 3.0: Application with persons with acquired apraxia of speech and aphasia. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 28: , 895–904.

[65] 

Strand E , Duffy JR , Clark HM , Josephs K (2017) The Apraxia of Speech Rating Scale: A tool for diagnosis and description of apraxia of speech (Version 3.0).

[66] 

Assal F , Laganaro M , Remund CD , Ragno Paquier C ((2012) ) Progressive crossed-apraxia of speech as a first manifestation of a probable corticobasal degeneration. Behav Neurol 25: , 285–289.

[67] 

Mazzon G , Menichelli A , Fabretto A , Cattaruzza T , Manganotti P ((2018) ) A new MAPT deletion in a case of speech apraxia leading to corticobasal syndrome. Neurocase 24: , 140–144.

[68] 

Marshall CR , Guerreiro R , Thust S , Fletcher P , Rohrer JD , Fox NC ((2015) ) A novel MAPT mutation causing corticobasal syndrome led by progressive apraxia of speech. J Alzheimers Dis 48: , 923–926.

[69] 

Sanchez-Valle R , Forman MS , Miller BL , Gorno-Tempini ML ((2006) ) From progressive nonfluent aphasia to corticobasal syndrome: A case report of corticobasal degeneration. Neurocase 12: , 355–359.

[70] 

Parjane N , Cho S , Ash S , Cousins KQA , Shellikeri S , Liberman M , Shaw LM , Irwin DJ , Grossman M , Nevler N ((2021) ) Digital speech analysis in progressive supranuclear palsy and corticobasal syndromes. J Alzheimers Dis 82: , 33–45.

[71] 

Labov W , Ash S , Boberg C (2006) The Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, Phonology and Sound Change, Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin.