Differences in reproducibility of gait variability and fractal dynamics according to walking duration
Abstract
BACKGROUND:
Gait variability and fractal dynamics may be affected by the walking duration.
OBJECTIVE:
The purpose of this study is to examine the reproducibility of stride time while walking on a self-paced treadmill.
METHODS:
Fifteen young and healthy subjects walked on the treadmill for 10 minutes. Three to eight minutes duration of the data were used to compare the trial-to-trial and day-to-day reproducibility of the average, variability, and fractal dynamics of stride time.
RESULTS:
The results show that all variables had high trial-to-trial reproducibility. In the day-to-day results, the average walking speed and mean stride time showed reproducibility without regard for duration, but the variability and gait fractal dynamics showed differences in reproducibility according to duration. The variability and fractal dynamics showed better reproducibility in less than 5 minutes and over time, respectively. However, both variables generally showed improved reproducibility when average data from two to three rounds were used.
CONCLUSION:
Based on the results of this study, it is proposed that variability should be examined using data of 5 min or less, and fractal dynamics should be examined using 5 min or more of repeated data when performing walking tests from a gait dynamics perspective.
1.Introduction
Fluctuations in continuous human walking data are not simply caused by irregular data. Walking studies that consider this phenomenon are known as “gait dynamics studies,” and long-range correlation is a widely used method for representing this phenomenon quantitatively [1]. In particular, there have been studies on using detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) to quantify long-range correlations in gait interval time data. Following these studies, there have been various studies on gait fractal dynamics, and this phenomenon has been used in studies up to the present time. There have also been studies on changes in the fractal dynamics of gait interval times resulting from neuro-diseases and walking conditions [1, 2], and the analysis methods for this research have been supplemented and improved [3]. Based on these various previous studies, it is known that the long-range correlation characteristics of gait fractal dynamics can disappear because of the artificial (intended) adjustment of gait variables [4, 5, 6], and gait fractal dynamics can change because of neuro-diseases and reductions in body balance caused by aging [1, 7, 8]. It is also generally known that there is a need for walking data gathered over a long time to analyze gait dynamics [9].
Treadmills have been used in previous gait studies as a means of overcoming restrictions on experiment equipment and space. Conventional treadmills have a fixed belt speed and a limited gait speed for the walker. However, various forms of self-paced treadmill are currently being used to improve experimental environments. From a gait dynamics perspective, it is expected that self-paced treadmills will provide conditions that are somewhat more similar to over-ground walking than conventional treadmills [6, 10, 11, 12, 13]. However, it is necessary to perform additional studies on this matter.
In the case of elderly people suffering from a disease or falling, it is not easy to participate in a long walking experiments. Therefore, it is necessary to confirm the minimum walking time that is necessary to analyze gait dynamics. In a previous study by Pierrynowski et al. on walking duration (3–8 min) and the reproducibility of gait fractal dynamics while walking on a conventional treadmill, the highest trial-to-trial day and day-to-day reproducibility was seen in walking for 8 min. For shorter periods of walking duration, it was shown that reproducibility can be improved through repeated trials data, such as four trials of 3 min or three trials of 6 min [14]. Choi et al. reported on the trial-to-trial day and day-to-day reproducibility for a self-paced treadmill, but the analysis was not performed according to walking duration [10]. Therefore, there is a need to confirm the results regarding walking duration and reproducibility on a self-paced treadmill. The goal of this study is to examine trial-to-trial and day-to-day reproducibility according to walking duration on a self-paced treadmill. In addition, this study aims to examine the reproducibility of gait variability and fractal dynamics according to walking duration.
2.Methods
Fifteen healthy male university students (ages 22
The participants performed three treadmill walking sessions of 10 min each at their preferred walking speed (Day 1). After three to four days, the same experiments were repeated (Day 2). Before the experiment, participants were given enough practice time for treadmill adaptation, and more than 10 min of break time was provided between each experiment. They were also instructed to stare forward while walking and to maintain a constant walking speed.
All walking experiments were conducted on a treadmill with single belt (RX9200S, DRAX, Korea) that automatically controls its belt speed depending on the walking speed of the participants [10]. The treadmill belt speed was stored in a PC at a sampling frequency of 10 Hz.
Two reflective markers were attached at both the toe and heel of the participants. A three-dimensional motion analysis system consisting of six infrared cameras was used to collect motion data while walking at a sampling frequency of 120 Hz (Motion Analysis Corps, USA). The foot velocity algorithm method was used to detect gait events such as heel contact. The stride time is calculated as the time between successive heel contacts [15].
From 10 min of acquired data, 8 min was analyzed by excluding the first and the last minute. To identify the reproducibility with the walking durations, the data were cut into 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8 min and compared.
For the analysis, the average walking speed and the mean stride time, stride time variability, and fractal dynamics of the stride time were compared. The average walking speed of the subjects was used as the average of the treadmill belt speed stored in the PC. The coefficient of variance (CV) was used for the variability of the stride time. The scaling exponent
To investigate the trial-to-trial and day-to-day reproducibility of the variables, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and standard error of measurement (SEM) were used. Type (3, k) is used for ICC, and SEM is as shown below. The meaning of ICC size can be interpreted as follows [10, 13, 18]:
SEM | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Poor | Fair | Good | Excellent | |
ICC | 0.40 | 0.60 | 0.75 |
To confirm reproducibility by date (day-to-day), the averages of Trial 1, Trials 1 and 2, and Trials 1, 2, and 3 of each date were also checked. To investigate the differences between trials, repeated ANOVA were performed. SPSS Statistics version 25 (IBM Corp., Somers, NY, USA) was used in the statistical analysis. The statistical significance (
3.Results
3.1Trial-to-trial reproducibility
Table 1 presents the reproducibility results for the variables according to three walking trials on the same day (Day 1). For all variables, there is no significant difference between trials, and the reproducibility was high. Also, there is no significant difference according to the walking duration (3–8 min), and the reproducibility was high for all variables.
Table 1
Variables | Duration (min.) | Trial 1 | Trial 2 | Trial 3 | ANOVA | ICC |
| SEM |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Walking speed | 3 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.17) | 1.44 (0.17) | 0.52 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.06 |
(m/sec) | 4 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.17) | 1.43 (0.17) | 0.35 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.06 |
5 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.17) | 1.43 (0.17) | 0.35 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |
6 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.17) | 1.44 (0.18) | 0.46 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |
7 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.18) | 1.44 (0.18) | 0.43 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |
8 | 1.42 (0.15) | 1.44 (0.18) | 1.44 (0.18) | 0.32 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |
Stride time | ||||||||
Mean (sec) | 3 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.06) | 1.06 (0.06) | 0.23 | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.01 |
4 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.06) | 1.06 (0.06) | 0.32 | 0.97 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
5 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.06) | 1.06 (0.06) | 0.25 | 0.97 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
6 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.06) | 1.06 (0.06) | 0.32 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
7 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.05) | 1.06 (0.05) | 0.29 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
8 | 1.06 (0.05) | 1.07 (0.06) | 1.06 (0.06) | 0.21 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
CV (%) | 3 | 1.43 (0.28) | 1.36 (0.39) | 1.32 (0.25) | 0.57 | 0.60 | 0.05 | 0.19 |
4 | 1.47 (0.29) | 1.43 (0.36) | 1.37 (0.27) | 0.41 | 0.97 | 0.00 | 0.05 | |
5 | 1.48 (0.28) | 1.46 (0.37) | 1.38 (0.26) | 0.41 | 0.78 | 0.01 | 0.14 | |
6 | 1.49 (0.29) | 1.47 (0.38) | 1.47 (0.35) | 0.97 | 0.78 | 0.01 | 0.16 | |
7 | 1.52 (0.35) | 1.45 (0.34) | 1.52 (0.39) | 0.64 | 0.75 | 0.02 | 0.18 | |
8 | 1.54 (0.33) | 1.45 (0.35) | 1.54 (0.41) | 0.44 | 0.77 | 0.01 | 0.17 | |
DFA ( | 3 | 0.83 (0.13) | 0.76 (0.15) | 0.76 (0.14) | 0.23 | 0.62 | 0.04 | 0.09 |
4 | 0.82 (0.11) | 0.79 (0.15) | 0.80 (0.11) | 0.52 | 0.69 | 0.01 | 0.07 | |
5 | 0.84 (0.11) | 0.80 (0.12) | 0.82 (0.10) | 0.42 | 0.63 | 0.02 | 0.07 | |
6 | 0.84 (0.12) | 0.82 (0.12) | 0.84 (0.10) | 0.51 | 0.73 | 0.01 | 0.06 | |
7 | 0.85 (0.12) | 0.82 (0.13) | 0.85 (0.10) | 0.63 | 0.76 | 0.00 | 0.06 | |
8 | 0.86 (0.12) | 0.84 (0.11) | 0.87 (0.11) | 0.47 | 0.73 | 0.01 | 0.06 |
Bold letters are
Table 2
Variables | Duration | Use of trials | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(min.) | 1 trial | Average of 2 trials | Average of 3 trials | |||||||
ICC |
| SEM | ICC |
| SEM | ICC |
| SEM | ||
Walking speed | 3 | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 |
(m/sec) | 4 | 0.95 | 0.00 | 0.12 | 0.97 | 0.00 | 0.09 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 |
5 | 0.95 | 0.00 | 0.12 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | |
6 | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | |
7 | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.1 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | |
8 | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.1 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.98 | 0.00 | 0.09 | |
Stride time | ||||||||||
Mean (sec) | 3 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.9 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 |
4 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.91 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
5 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.92 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
6 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.92 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
7 | 0.86 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
8 | 0.88 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.94 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
CV (%) | 3 | 0.71 | 0.04 | 0.15 | 0.84 | 0.00 | 0.1 | 0.94 | 0.00 | 0.06 |
4 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.91 | 0.00 | 0.09 | 0.93 | 0.00 | 0.07 | |
5 | 0.77 | 0.01 | 0.14 | 0.81 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.89 | 0.00 | 0.08 | |
6 | 0.63 | 0.07 | 0.18 | 0.86 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.89 | 0.00 | 0.09 | |
7 | 0.45 | 0.20 | 0.25 | 0.83 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.89 | 0.00 | 0.09 | |
8 | 0.54 | 0.12 | 0.23 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.12 | 0.84 | 0.01 | 0.12 | |
DFA ( | 3 | 0.17 | 0.37 | 0.13 | 0.43 | 0.22 | 0.08 | 0.73 | 0.04 | 0.06 |
4 | 0.48 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.61 | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.84 | 0.01 | 0.04 | |
5 | 0.36 | 0.15 | 0.1 | 0.54 | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.81 | 0.01 | 0.04 | |
6 | 0.66 | 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.79 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.84 | 0.01 | 0.04 | |
7 | 0.62 | 0.03 | 0.08 | 0.74 | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.78 | 0.02 | 0.05 | |
8 | 0.63 | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.71 | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.73 | 0.03 | 0.05 |
Bold letters are
3.2Day-to-day reproducibility
Table 2 shows the reproducibility results by date for three trials performed on Days 1 and 2. The average walking speed and the mean stride time showed reproducibility by date without regard to the number of trials or the walking duration. However, the CV and DFA results generally showed good reproducibility only when there were an average of two trials or more. When the results of one trial were compared, the CV only showed reproducibility when the duration was 5 min or less, and the DFA showed reproducibility only when the duration was 6 min or more (Fig. 1).
Figure 1.
4.Discussion
The trial-to-trial and day-to-day reproducibility of the average walking speed and the mean, variability, and gait fractal dynamics of the stride time when young adults walked on a self-paced treadmill were examined. Also it was examined the difference in these variables according to a walking duration of 3–8 min. The results can be summarized as follows. The trial-to-trial day results generally showed good reproducibility with a high ICC and low SEM for all variables. For the day-to-day results, the reproducibility of the average walking speed and the mean stride time was constant regardless of the duration, but the reproducibility of the variability and the gait fractal dynamics of the stride time varied according to the duration. In a comparison using single walking trials on different dates, the variability showed reproducibility for relatively short durations of 5 min or less, while the fractal dynamics had good reproducibility only for long duration data of 6 min or more. However, when the average data from two to three trials were used for each variable, the reproducibility generally improved (Table 2, Fig. 1).
Wiens et al. used 14-min data from a self-paced treadmill to examine the day-to-day reproducibility of step time variability and fractal dynamics, as well as the differences compared with a conventional treadmill. In the results of day-to-day, the variability and fractal dynamics were represented low reproducibility [13]. When the single-trial experiment data were compared for each date, the variability and fractal dynamics were represented relatively low reproducibility, and this corresponded to the results of this study. However, the step time used in the previous paper was different from the stride time, and no repeated experiment was performed. Pierrynowski et al. examined the reproducibility of gait fractal dynamics on a conventional treadmill [14]. According to our results, trial-to-trial reproducibility was consistent regardless of the walking duration, but the day-to-day reproducibility was affected by the duration (3
Another notable result of this study is that the variability and fractal dynamic characteristics showed opposite to each other around 5
This study has several limitations. First, this study was performed on only healthy adults, and senior citizens or patient with any diseases may produce different results. Also, the walking speed (i.e., preferred walking speed) and variables (i.e., stride time) were limited by the experiment conditions. As with the previous experiments that were compared above, it can be expected that there will be some degree of relationship with the step time and spatial data (length, width, etc.) results, but the same results cannot be guaranteed. Finally, a standardized algorithm for self-paced treadmills has not been proposed yet, and, therefore, the results may have been influenced by the speed control algorithm.
5.Conclusions
The trial-to-trial day and day-to-day reproducibility of young adults’ average walking speed and the mean, variability, and fractal dynamics of their stride time when walking on a self-paced treadmill were examined. The differences in each variable according to a walking duration of 3–8 min were also examined. The results can be summarized as follows: In the trial-to-trial day results, all variables generally showed good reproducibility with a good ICC and a small SEM. In the day-to-day results, the average walking speed and the mean stride time reproducibility was consistent regardless of duration, but the variability and the fractal dynamics of the stride time showed differences in reproducibility according to duration. In a comparison using single walking trials on different dates, the variability showed reproducibility for relatively brief durations of 5 min or less, while the fractal dynamics had good reproducibility only for long-duration data of 6 min or more. However, when the averages of the data from two to three trials were used, the reproducibility generally improved for all variables. Based on the results of this study, it is proposed that variability should be examined using 5 min or less of data, and fractal dynamics should be examined by repeatedly measuring 5 min or more of data when performing walking tests from a gait dynamics perspective.
Acknowledgments
This paper was supported by Konkuk University in 2018.
Conflict of interest
None to report.
References
[1] | Hausdorff JM. Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum Mov Sci. (2007) ; 26: : 555-589. |
[2] | Terrier P, Dériaz O. Persistent and anti-persistent pattern in stride-to-stride variability of treadmill walking: influence of rhythmic auditory cueing. Hum Mov Sci. (2012) ; 31: (6): 1585-1597. |
[3] | Almurad ZMH, Delignières D. Evenly spacing in detrended fluctuation analysis. Phys A Stat Mech Appl. (2016) ; 451: : 63-69. |
[4] | Dingwell JB, John J, Cusumano JP. Do Humans Optimally Exploit Redundancy to Control Step Variability in Walking? PLoS Comput Biol. (2010) ; 6: (7): e1000856. |
[5] | Hausdorff JM, Purdon PL, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J Appl Physiol. (1996) ; 80: : 1448-1457. |
[6] | Choi JS, Kang DW, Seo JW, Tack GR. Fractal fluctuations in spatiotemporal variables when walking on a self-paced treadmill. J Biomech. (2017) ; 65: : 154-160. |
[7] | Hausdorff JM, Mitchell SL, Firtion R, Peng CK, Cudkowicz ME, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Altered fractal dynamics of gait: reduced stride-interval correlations with aging and Huntington’s disease. J Appl Physiol. (1997) ; 82: : 262-269. |
[8] | Marmelat V, Meidinger RL. Fractal analysis of gait in people with Parkinson’s disease: three minutes is not enough. Gait Posture. (2019) ; 70: : 229-234. |
[9] | Kirchner M, Schubert P, Liebherr M, Haas CT. Detrended fluctuation analysis and adaptive fractal analysis of stride time data in parkinson’s disease: stitching together short gait trials. PLoS One. (2014) ; 9: (1): e85787. |
[10] | Choi J-S, Kang D-W, Seo J-W, Tack G-R. Reliability of the walking speed and gait dynamics variables while walking on a feedback-controlled treadmill. J Biomech. (2015) ; 48: : 1336-1339. |
[11] | Minetti AE, Boldrini L, Brusamolin L, Zamparo P, McKee T. A feedback-controlled treadmill (treadmill-on-demand) and the spontaneous speed of walking and running in humans. J Appl Physiol. (2003) ; 95: : 838-843. |
[12] | Sloot LH, van der Krogt MM, Harlaar J. Self-paced versus fixed speed treadmill walking. Gait Posture. (2014) ; 39: : 478-484. |
[13] | Wiens C, Denton W, Schieber MN, Hartley R, Marmelat V, Myers SA, Yentes JM. Walking speed and spatiotemporal step mean measures are reliable during feedback-controlled treadmill walking; however, spatiotemporal step variability is not reliable. J Biomech. (2019) ; 83: : 221-226. |
[14] | Pierrynowski MR, Gross A, Miles M, Galea V, McLaughlin L, McPhee C. Reliability of the long-range power-law correlations obtained from the bilateral stride intervals in asymptomatic volunteers whilst treadmill walking. Gait Posture. (2005) ; 22: : 46-50. |
[15] | O’Connor CM, Thorpe SK, O’Malley MJ, Vaughan CL. Automatic detection of gait events using kinematic data. Gait Posture. (2007) ; 25: (3): 469-474. |
[16] | Peng CK, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL. Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. Chaos. (1995) ; 5: : 82-87. |
[17] | Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP. Re-interpreting detrended fluctuation analyses of stride-to-stride variability in human walking. Gait Posture. (2010) ; 32: : 348-353. |
[18] | Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. (1994) ; 6: : 284. |
[19] | Roemmich R, Zeilman P, Vaillancourt D, Okun M, Hass C. Gait variability magnitude but not structure is altered in essential tremor. J Biomech. (2013) ; 46: (15): 2682-2687. |