You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

High IQ in Early Adulthood Is Associated with Parkinson’s Disease

Abstract

Background:

High education level and high occupational complexity have been implicated as risk factors for Parkinson’s disease (PD).

Objective:

The objective was to determine whether cognitive capacity, measured as IQ, in early adulthood is associated with the subsequent development of PD.

Method:

Data on IQ were retrieved from the Swedish Military Service Conscription Registry, comprising Swedish males who enlisted for military service in the period 1968–1993 (N = 1,319,235). After exclusion, 1,189,134 subjects in total were included in the present study. Individuals who later developed PD (N = 1,724) were identified using the Swedish National Patient Register and the Swedish Cause of Death Register.

Results:

High education level was associated with PD. High IQ was associated with PD (p < 0.0001), both when analyzed as a continuous variable and when divided into three categories. The hazard ratio for the high IQ category compared to the low IQ category was 1.35 (95% confidence interval 1.17–1.55). Strong test results on the subtests, measuring verbal, logic, visuospatial and technical abilities, were also associated with PD. In a subgroup, smoking was inversely associated with PD, as well as with IQ.

Conclusions:

This study identifies high IQ to be a risk factor for PD.

INTRODUCTION

It has been reported that high level of education is a risk factor for Parkinson’s disease (PD) [1, 2]. Furthermore, in a twins study, PD was more common in individuals who engaged in work of high complexity involving data and people [3].

Many studies have identified associations between PD and specific occupations, for example teaching and healthcare services, clerkship, social science, law and library jobs, and farming, many but not all of which require a high level of education [4–7]. In contrast, the performance of outdoor work has been shown to be inversely associated with PD [8, 9]. Most of the discussions regarding associations between PD and education and occupations have focused on the possibility that certain occupations may increase the risk for PD by exposure to environmental factors, such as toxins (e.g., in farming) or infections (e.g., in teaching and healthcare services).

However, since IQ is a strong predictor of time spent in education and later job status, cognitive performance might be part of the etiology for the association between high-level education and PD. To test this hypothesis, data on cognitive capacity were retrieved from the Swedish Military Service Conscription Registry, which comprises Swedish males who enlisted for military service in the period 1968–1993 (N = 1,319,235). Individuals who later developed PD (N = 1,724) were identified in the Swedish National Hospital Discharge Register.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study population

We performed an exploratory, population-based, prospective study of late-adolescent men who underwent compulsory military conscription examinations. Every Swedish resident is assigned a unique personal identification number, making linkage between different registers possible. The present study is based on data retrieved from the Swedish Military Service Conscription Registry, comprising all Swedish males who were born in the period of 1949–1975, who enlisted for military service in the period of 1968–1993 (N = 1,319,235). During that period, military conscription was compulsory and only 2% –3% of all Swedish men were exempted from conscription, in most cases due to severe mental or physical disorders, or imprisonment. Among the subjects who developed PD during the follow-up, only those who were diagnosed at or after the age of 40 years were included in the present study. The exclusion criteria were: late allocated personal identity number (i.e., personal identity number reallocated from deceased person), female sex, >25 years of age at conscription, missing data regarding the conscription test center, or PD diagnosis, death or emigration before 40 years of age. The remaining 1,189,134 subjects included in the present study were born in the period 1949–1975 and enlisted for military service in the period 1968–1993 (Fig. 1). In total, 1,724 of the included subjects developed PD later in life.

Fig. 1

Flow chart illustrating the exclusion criteria, number of diagnoses of Parkinson’s disease (PD), and follow-up time of the Swedish male conscript study population in the period 1968-1993, based on the recommendations in Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) [38].

Flow chart illustrating the exclusion criteria, number of diagnoses of Parkinson’s disease (PD), and follow-up time of the Swedish male conscript study population in the period 1968-1993, based on the recommendations in Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) [38].

Conscription register data

The conscripts underwent standardized physical and cognitive examinations conducted by a psychologist and a physician at one of six conscription centers in Sweden (Southern, Western, Eastern, Central, Northern Lower, and Northern Upper).

IQ measurements

The cognitive abilities were assessed at conscription using the Swedish Enlistment Battery (SEB). Two different versions of the SEB were used during the study period: SEB67 and SEB80. They consisted of four subtests that measured verbal, logical, visuospatial, and technical capabilities, the scores for which were then summed to give a global IQ score. The outcome of each subtest and the global IQ score were ranked on a scale of 1–9, with 1 being the lowest scoring group and 9 the highest scoring group. The SEB67 battery was used during the period of 1968–1979. “Instructions” was a logical test comprising 40 items that measured the ability to understand instructions and apply them to solve a problem. “Concept Discrimination” consisted of 40 items and examined verbal ability by having the conscript choose which word out of five alternatives did not agree with the others conceptually. “Paper Form Board” was a visuospatial test with 25 items, containing questions about two-dimensional puzzles. “Technical Comprehension” was a 52-item test comprising illustrated technical and physical problems.

The SEB80 battery was used during the period 1980–1993. “Instructions” was retained with some improvements. “Concept Discrimination” was replaced by “Synonyms”, which involved 40 items to assessing vocabulary by letting the conscript identify which out of four alternatives was the synonym of a given word. “Paper Form Board” was replaced by another visuospatial test called “Metal Folding”. This 40-item test measured geometrical perception by having the conscript identify the correct three-dimensional object from a series of two-dimensional drawings. “Technical Comprehension” was modified to measure knowledge of mathematics, physics and chemistry, consisting of 40 items.

The 1969–1970 cohort

This sub-cohort consisted of 49,321 Swedish males, born in the period 1949–1951, who were conscripted for compulsory military service in the period 1969–1970. Apart from the standard physical and cognitive examination, the 1969–1970 conscripts participated in a survey designed to collect information on social background, behavior and social adjustment, and substance use, e.g., alcohol consumption and tobacco smoking. The conscripts were asked to report their smoking habits according to one of the following five levels: non-smoker, 1–5 cigarettes per day, 6–10 cigarettes per day, 11–20 cigarettes per day, and >20 cigarettes per day.

Educational level

Information on the conscripts’ levels of education and those of their parents were obtained from the Longitudinal Integration database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies (LISA) [40]. This database, which was started in 1990 and includes all registered residents aged 16 years and older, integrates data from the labor market and educational and social sectors. Every 5 years between 1960 and 1990, the government agency Statistics Sweden sent out a questionnaire to collect data on variables such as age, sex, civil status, country of birth, citizenship, education, employment, and occupation from all Swedish adults aged ≥16 years. Swedish law stipulated that these surveys had to be completed and this census had a response rate of over 98%. Most data in LISA from 1990 have other sources and are not self-reported; most of the variables are collected automatically from schools and institutions. There is a file for every individual which is updated every year, containing the highest educational level attained. In the analysis, education was classified into three categories (educational levels): pre-high school up to 9 years (low); high school up to 12 years (medium); and university ≥2 years and postgraduate (high). Each category was divided in groups based on the period in which the subject was conscripted: conscription before 1975, in the period 1975–1984, and in the period 1985–1993. For the parents of the conscripts, the highest level of education attained by either parent was used.

Outcomes

Data sources

Data from the Swedish Military Service Conscription Register were linked to the Swedish National Patient Register (NPR) and the Swedish Cause of Death Register. The Swedish National Inpatient Register (IPR), also called the Swedish Hospital Discharge Register, is a part of the NPR and was initiated in 1968. The coverage of IPR gradually increased until 1986 and has complete national coverage since 1987. Since 2001, NPR also contains information on out-patient visits. Deaths were identified by linkage with the Swedish Cause of Death Register, which is updated annually based on the issuance of death certificates and covers virtually all deaths since 1961.

PD diagnoses

PD diagnoses in the study cohort through December 31, 2016, were acquired using the conscripts’ personal identification numbers from the NPR or the Swedish Cause of Death Register. From 1987 to 1996, the International Classification of Diseases, ninth and tenth revisions (ICD-9 and ICD-10) were in use. Diagnoses of PD were coded as 332A in records using the Swedish version of the ICD-9 (1987–1997) and as G20 in records using the ICD-10 (1998–2012). The NPR is administered by the Center for Epidemiology of Sweden’s National Board of Health and Welfare. Diagnoses recorded in the NPR have shown a high degree of validity, with positive predictive values of 85% –97% [39].

Statistical analysis

The follow-up period started at the date of conscription (baseline) and subjects were followed until the time of: 1) first hospitalization for PD or hospital-based outpatient clinic contact for PD; 2) death; 3) emigration; or 4) the end of follow-up, on December 31, 2016 (follow-up: minimum, 15 years; maximum, 48 years). Cox proportional hazards models were used to assess the influences of presumptive predictors on PD diagnosis. Tests based on scaled Schoenfeld residuals showed no significant violations of the proportional hazards assumption.

Intelligence scores were scaled on a nine-point standard scale (stanine = standard nine) and then divided into three groups: low (stanine score 1–3); medium (4–6); and high (7–9), with the latter used as the reference. Education and parental education was trichotomized (low, medium, and high), as described earlier. Conscription year and test center were adjusted for in all the regression models, to account for temporal differences and procedural differences across the participating sites. A cubic restricted spline with knots at the 5th, 35th, 65th and 95th percentiles was used for conscription year. Furthermore, age at conscription was adjusted so as to be linear due to the limited range. The statistical calculations were performed with the SAS ver. 9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

The Ethics Committee of the University of Gothenburg and Confidentiality Clearance at Statistics Sweden approved the study. The requirement for informed consent was waived for the current study as it entails a secondary analysis of existing data. The investigation conforms to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki in relation to ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects.

RESULTS

Of the 1,319,235 conscripts in the present study, 1,724 were diagnosed with PD during the 48 years of follow-up (Fig. 1). Almost all cases (N = 1718) were identified by the NPR, of which 1561 were identified by the outpatient hospital register and 157 by the inpatient hospital register. A total of 6 cases were identified by the Cause of death register. In total, 130,101 conscripts were excluded from the analyses. Overall, 40,468,829 person-years of follow-up were included in the study. The observed median age at PD diagnosis was 55 years (range, 40–66 years).

The distributions of educational levels of the conscripts and of their parents are shown in Table 1. Parental education and conscript education was correlated (r = 0.4, p < 0.0001) as well as parental education and conscript IQ (r = 0.5 p < 0.0001). We evaluated the education levels of the conscripts and their parents, smoking histories, and IQ levels in relation to the risk of developing PD. Cox proportional hazards models show that education was prospectively associated with PD in analyses adjusted for age, conscription year and conscription test center with HR 1.2 (95% CI 1.01–1.32) for high school and HR 1.4 (95% CI 1.19–1-58) for University and postgraduate (both compared to pre-high school). Corresponding Cox regressions for the highest parental educational level showed no statistically significant differences. When considered as a continuous variable, Global IQ and all four subtests of IQ taken individually were associated with PD (Table 2). High scores on IQ tests were associated with an increased risk of being diagnosed with PD later in life. When dividing the test scores into three categories (1–3, 4–6, 7–9), high Global IQ at conscription was associated with a 1.3- fold (95% CI 1.17–1.55) increase in risk for PD later in life, as compared to low Global IQ. High test scores on the three subtests that measured logical, verbal, and technical IQ levels were associated with 1.2-fold (95% CI 1.02–1.37), 1.3- fold (95% CI 1.15–1.55), and 1.3-fold (95% CI 1.09–1.45) increased risk for PD, respectively. In the analyses that adjusted for age at conscription and birth year, higher IQ level was associated with an increase in the risk for PD. Parental education level did not affect the results.

Table 1

Distribution and percentages of male conscripts by their IQ, their educational level and the educational level of their parents

Conscripts’ educationParental education
IQ 1–3IQ 4–6IQ 7–9IQ 1–3IQ 4–6IQ 7–9
(N = 235,831)(N = 620,232)(N = 278,339)(N = 234,103)(N = 617,063)(N = 276,705)
Pre-high school (≤9 years)
  Conscription<197522,890 (47.1%)31,437 (24.3%)5,244 (7.8%)33,101 (69.7%)72,016 (56.8%)26,847 (40.5%)
  Conscription 1975–198431,483 (33.9%)36,748 (15.1%)4,458 (4.2%)49,671 (53.6%)95,537 (39.3%)25,950 (24.4%)
  Conscription 1985–199319,499 (20.7%)14,945 (6.0%)1,449 (1.4%)32,870 (35.0%)52,810 (21.4%)11,425 (11.0%)
High school (≤12 years)
  Conscription before 197523,133 (47.6%)67,530 (52.3%)23,576 (34.8%)12,394 (26.1%)42,328 (33.4%)24,246 (36.5%)
  Conscription 1975–198456,966 (61.3%)149,024 (61.1%)34,676 (32.6%)35,999 (38.9%)104,784 (43.1%)40,986 (38.6%)
  Conscription 1985–199367,918 (72.1%)151,627 (61.4%)24,453 (23.5%)49,835 (53.0%)126,180 (51.1%)40,487 (38.9%)
University (≥2 years)
  and postgraduate
  Conscription before 19752,624 (5.4%)30,265 (23.4%)38,839 (57.4%)1,979 (4.2%)12,408 (9.8%)15,261 (23.0%)
  Conscription 1975–19844,509 (4.9%)58,199 (23.9%)67,387 (63.3%)6,938 (7.5%)43,079 (17.7%)39,299 (37.0%)
  Conscription 1985–19936,809 (7.2%)80,457 (32.6%)78,257 (75.1%)11,316 (12.0%)67,921 (27.5%)52,204 (50.1%)

All percentages are column percentages by each conscription time period. IQ groups: low (1–3); medium (4–6); and high (7–9).

Table 2

Hazard ratios for Parkinson’s disease in relation to IQ, education and parental education

Hazard ratio95% CIp-value
A
Model 1:
  Global IQ1.051.03–1.08<.0001
  Subtest 1: Logical IQ1.041.01–1.060.008
  Subtest 2: Verbal IQ1.051.03–1.080.0001
  Subest 3: Visuospatial IQ1.041.01–1.070.005
  Subtest 4: Technical IQ1.041.02–1.070.002
B
Model 1:
  Global IQ1.351.17–1.55<.0001
  Subest 1: Logical IQ1.181.02–1.370.03
  Subest 2: Verbal IQ1.341.15–1.550.0001
  Subtest 3: Visuospatial IQ1.150.99–1.340.07
  Subtest 4: Technical IQ1.261.09–1.450.001
Model 2:
  Global IQ1.221.03–1.430.02
Model 3:
  Global IQ1.371.18–1.60<.0001
C
  Education1.371.19–1.58<.0001
D
  Parental education1.060.92–1.230.4

A) IQ test results were used as a continuous variable in Model 1, with baseline adjustments (conscription year, age at conscription and test center). B) Intelligence scores were collapsed into three groups, whereby the lowest scoring group was compared to the highest scoring group. The scores from the four subtests were summed to derive the Global IQ value. Model 1 is analyzed with baseline adjustments (conscription year, age at conscription and test center). Model 2 has baseline adjustments plus adjustments for the conscript’s education. Model 3 has baseline adjustments plus adjustments for the conscript’s parental education.

The descriptive data regarding smoking retrieved from the 1969–1970 sub-cohort is shown in Table 3. Of the 49,571 men evaluated at conscription during 1969 and 1970, there were 256 individuals who later received a diagnosis of PD. The PD cases reported smoking significantly fewer cigarettes at conscription than did the non-PD individuals. Table 4 presents descriptive information about IQ and smoking. Smoking prevalence significantly decreased as Global IQ test scores increased (p < 0.0001), with a Spearman correlation coefficient of –0.15. Fifty-two percent of conscripts with high Global IQ test scores (stanine 7–9) reported to be non-smokers, whereas 32% of the conscripts with low Global IQ test score (stanine 1–3) reported to be non-smokers. In the 1969–1970 sub-cohort, high IQ (compared to low) at conscription was not associated with later diagnosis of PD, HR 1.16 (95% CI 0.82–1.64) without adjustments and 1.05 (95% CI 0.73–1.50) with adjustment for smoking.

Table 3

Smoking habits at conscription and risk for Parkinson’s disease (PD)

Individuals (N = 49,514)Incidence rate per 100,000 person-yearsHazard ratio95% CIp-value
No smoking20,49615.2Reference
1–5 cigarettes/day5,5669.70.660.43–1.000.05
6–10 cigarettes/day10,2888.00.520.36–0.750.0005
11–20 cigarettes/day11,3739.80.640.46–0.890.007
>20 cigarettes/day1,7917.50.490.22–1.120.09

Data from an investigation of smoking habits of men enlisted for military service in 1969 and 1970.

Table 4

Smoking habits and global intelligence levels of the conscripts

IQ>20 cig./day11–20 cig./day6–10 cig./day1–5 cig./dayNo smoking
Low510 (5.5%)2,413 (25.7%)2,319 (24.7%)1,163 (12.4%)2,974 (31.7%)
Average900 (3.7%)6,135 (25.1%)5,418 (22.1%)2,704 (11.0%)9,314 (38.1%)
High381 (2.4%)2,825 (18.0%)2,551 (16.3%)1,699 (10.9%)8,208 (52.4%)

Data from an investigation of smoking habits of men enlisted for military service in 1969 and 1970. The correlation coefficient is 0.15 (Spearman), p < 0.0001.

DISCUSSION

The main finding of the present study is that a high IQ is associated with an increased risk for PD (Table 2). We confirm the previously reported association between high education level and PD. The association between a high level of education and PD could be due to a high IQ level. It is well-established that high IQ and education level are correlated, at least in later generations in which education is not so dependent upon parental socioeconomic factors.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to report the association of high IQ and the risk of PD. More usually the opposite is the case: low education level, low IQ, and low socioeconomic status are associated with disease. Low IQ scores have been associated with cardiovascular events and disease, suicide, excessive alcohol consumption, and schizophrenia [10–19]. In line with the hypothesis that higher intelligence affects health outcomes through superior ability to prevent and manage disease, those with higher intelligence should be at lower risk. However, in the case of PD, we found the opposite.

Since IQ is to a major extent inherited [20] we investigated the levels of education of the parents of our subjects. Whereas, no information is available on parental IQ levels, there is information on parental education levels, which can be considered as a proxy of socioeconomic level and childhood environment. To our surprise, there was no significant association between parental education level and the presence of PD of the conscript. A probable explanation for this lack of association is that for most of the conscripts who later developed PD, their parents were born in the 1920s and 1930s. A high level of education was still quite rare at that time, which is why education is not a good reflection on IQ for those individuals, because it is more likely to reflect socioeconomic status than IQ or a person’s ability to attain a high education.

Previous studies have shown that individuals with low cognitive ability in early life are more likely to become smokers [21], less likely to stop smoking [22], and drink more alcohol [23] than individuals with higher cognitive ability. Since smoking shows a robust negative association with PD [24–28], we investigated the associations between smoking and PD and IQ in a sub-cohort of the conscripts. We confirm the negative association of smoking with PD (Table 3). In addition, we show (Table 4) an inverse association of smoking with IQ. However, IQ at conscription was not associated with later diagnosis of PD. Due to the low number of PD cases in the 1969–1970 sub-cohort (n = 256), there is a lack of power and the precision of this analysis is limited.

It has been discussed extensively whether the association between smoking and PD is due to cytoprotection or reversed causality, in the sense that smokers are more likely to quit smoking during prodromal PD symptoms or that the prodromal PD personality tends to choose not to smoke [25, 26]. Many experimental animal studies have suggested that nicotine is cytoprotective, while other substances present in cigarette smoke have also been proposed to be cytoprotective [29]. Also, a study on twins suggests a protective effect of smoking [41]. However, several findings speak against cytoprotection as a causal factor for the association with PD. One is that the rate of progression of symptoms is not modified by smoking in patients with PD [30–32]. Another aspect is that in a large prospective study, no difference was observed for the age at onset of PD between ever-smokers and never-smokers [33]. This was also reported in a study on moist smokeless tobacco [34]. Finally, and most compelling, is the finding that nicotine patch treatment did not slow the progression of PD symptoms in humans [35, 36]. It is possible that smoking is a proxy of some other factor associated with IQ. In another twin study, the protective association of smoking is less-pronounced, indicating that some underlying genetic or environmental factors are of importance [37]. IQ itself would probably also influence the individual’s exposure of environmental factors, such as infections, toxins, trauma etcetera during life, all factors that have been suggested to be of importance for the etiology of PD (for review see Wirdefeldt et al. [27]). The IQ level of an individual may influence the person’s occupation and to which extent the person has a sedentary lifestyle.

The unique strength of the present study is the use of a nationwide cohort, with a study sample consisting of 1,319,235 conscripts, the vast majority of all Swedish men born between 1951 and 1965. A sample size this large allows for a more accurate estimate of the association between IQ scores and PD risk. Having a study sample that represents such a large percentage of the population also eliminates the risk for selection bias. Many studies on early-life factors and later health outcomes rely on retrospective information collected at some point of the subject’s adult life which may introduce bias [42].

A limitation of the present study is that it included only men, thus, our results may not apply to women. Another potential limitation is that the diagnosis of PD may be better in highly educated people, which could affect the associations. Furthermore, although men in our study were followed for a maximum of 48 years, the average age at end of follow-up is relatively young (56 years) and the etiology of these PD diagnoses may differ from those occurring later in life.

In conclusion, the present study shows for the first time an association between high IQ test scores in adolescence and higher risk of being diagnosed with PD later in life. Additional studies on how IQ scores relate to PD risk are required to clarify this further.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflict of interest to report.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was funded by the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (FORTE), The LUA-ALF Foundation at Sahlgrenska University Hospital, and The Göteborg Medical Society.

REFERENCES

[1] 

Frigerio R , Elbaz A , Sanft KR , Peterson BJ , Bower JH , Ahlskog JE , Grossardt BR , de Andrade M , Maraganore DM , Rocca WA ((2005) ) Education and occupations preceding Parkinson disease: A population-based case-control study. Neurology 65: , 1575–1583.

[2] 

Li X , Sundquist J , Sundquist K ((2009) ) Socioeconomic and occupational groups and Parkinson’s disease: A nationwide study based on hospitalizations in Sweden. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 82: , 235–241.

[3] 

Valdés EG , Andel R , Sieurin J , Feldman AL , Edwards JD , Langstrom N , Gatz M , Wirdefeldt K ((2014) ) Occupational complexity and risk of Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One 9: , e106676.

[4] 

Tsui JK , Calne DB , Wang Y , Schulzer M , Marion SA ((1999) ) Occupational risk factors in Parkinson’s disease. Can J Public Health 90: , 334–337.

[5] 

Park J , Yoo CI , Sim CS , Kim JW , Yi Y , Jung KY , Chung SE , Kim Y ((2004) ) Occupations and Parkinson’s disease: A case-control study in South Korea. Ind Health 42: , 352–358.

[6] 

Teschke K , Marion SA , Tsui JK , Shen H , Rugbjerg K , Harris MA ((2014) ) Parkinson’s disease and occupation: Differences in associations by case identification method suggest referral bias. Am J Ind Med 57: , 163–171.

[7] 

Kirkey KL , Johnson CC , Rybicki BA , Peterson EL , Kortsha GX , Gorell JM ((2001) ) Occupational categories at risk for Parkinson’s disease. Am J Ind Med 39: , 564–571.

[8] 

Kenborg L , Lassen CF , Ritz B , Schernhammer ES , Hansen J , Gatto NM , Olsen JH ((2011) ) Outdoor work and risk for Parkinson’s disease: A population-based case-control study. Occup Environ Med 68: , 273–278.

[9] 

Kwon E , Gallagher LG , Searles Nielsen S , Franklin GM , Littell CT , Longstreth WT Jr, Swanson PD , Checkoway H ((2013) ) Parkinson’s disease and history of outdoor occupation. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 19: , 1164–1166.

[10] 

Lindgren M , Eriksson P , Rosengren A , Robertson J , Schiöler L , Schaufelberger M , Åberg D , Torén K , Waern M , Åberg M ((2018) ) Cognitive performance in late adolescence and long-term risk of early heart failure in Swedish men. Eur J Heart Fail 20: , 989–997.

[11] 

Hart CL , Taylor MD , Smith GD , Whalley LJ , Starr JM , Hole DJ , Wilson V Deary IJ ((2004) ) Childhood IQ and cardiovascular disease in adulthood: Prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Soc Sci Med 59: , 2131–2138.

[12] 

Batty GD , Mortensen EL , Nybo Andersen AM , Osler M. Batty GD ((2005) ) Childhood intelligence in relation to adult coronary heart disease and stroke risk: Evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 19: , 452–459.

[13] 

Silventoinen K , Modig-Wennerstad K , Tynelius P , Rasmussen F ((2007) ) Association between intelligence and coronary heart disease mortality: A population-based cohort study of 682 361 Swedish men. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil 14: , 555–560.

[14] 

Dobson KG , Chow CH , Morrison KM , Van Lieshout RJ ((2017) ) Associations between childhood cognition and cardiovascular events in adulthood: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Can J Cardiol 33: , 232–242.

[15] 

Gunnell D , Magnusson PK , Rasmussen F ((2005) ) Low intelligence test scores in 18 year old men and risk of suicide: Cohort study. BMJ 330: , 167.

[16] 

Hemmingsson T , Lundberg I , Diderichsen F , Allebeck P ((1998) ) Explanations of social class differences in alcoholism among young men. Soc Sci Med 47: , 1399–1405.

[17] 

Hemmingsson T , Melin B , Allebeck P , Lundberg I ((2006) ) The association between cognitive ability measured at ages 18-20 and mortality during 30 years of follow-up -a prospective observational study among Swedish males born 1949-51. Int J Epidemiol 35: , 665–670.

[18] 

David AS , Malmberg A , Brandt L , Allebeck P , Lewis G ((1997) ) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: A population-based cohort study. Psychol Med 27: , 1311–1323.

[19] 

Zammit S , Allebeck P , David AS , Dalman C , Hemmingsson T , Lundberg I , Lewis G ((2004) ) A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ Score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61: , 354–360.

[20] 

Åberg MA , Pedersen NL , Toren K , Svartengren M , Backstrand B , Johnsson T , Cooper-Kuhn CM , Åberg ND , Nilsson M , Kuhn HG ((2009) ) Cardiovascular fitness is associated with cognition in young adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: , 20906–20911.

[21] 

Martin LT , Fitzmaurice GM , Kindlon DJ , Buka SL ((2004) ) Cognitive performance in childhood and early adult illness: A prospective cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health 58: , 674–679.

[22] 

Taylor MD , Hart CL , Davey Smith G , Starr JM , Hole DJ , Whalley LJ , Wilson V , Deary IJ ((2003) ) Childhood mental ability and smoking cessation in adulthood: Prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 57: , 464–465.

[23] 

Batty GD , Deary IJ , Macintyre S ((2006) ) Childhood IQ and life course socioeconomic position in relation to alcohol induced hangovers in adulthood: The Aberdeen children of the 1950s study. J Epidemiol Community Health 60: , 872–874.

[24] 

Hernan MA , Takkouche B , Caamano-Isorna F , Gestal-Otero JJ ((2002) ) A meta-analysis of coffee drinking, cigarette smoking, and the risk of Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol 52: , 276–284.

[25] 

Allam MF , Campbell MJ , Del Castillo AS , Fernandez-Crehuet Navajas R ((2004) ) Parkinson’s disease protects against smoking? Behav Neurol 15: , 65–71.

[26] 

Allam MF , Campbell MJ , Hofman A , Del Castillo AS , Fernandez-Crehuet Navajas R ((2004) ) Smoking and Parkinson’s disease: Systematic review of prospective studies. Mov Disord 19: , 614–621.

[27] 

Wirdefeldt K , Adami HO , Cole P , Trichopoulos D , Mandel J ((2011) ) Epidemiology and etiology of Parkinson’s disease: A review of the evidence. Eur J Epidemiol 1, S, 26 Suppl: , 1–58.

[28] 

Li X , Li W , Liu G , Shen X , Tang Y ((2015) ) Association between cigarette smoking and Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 61: , 510–516.

[29] 

Quik M , Perez XA , Bordia T ((2012) ) Nicotine as a potential neuroprotective agent for Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 27: , 947–957.

[30] 

Golbe LO , Cody RA , Duvoisin RC ((1986) ) Smoking and Parkinson’s disease. Search for a dose-response relationship. Arch Neurol 43: , 774–778.

[31] 

Alves G , Kurz M , Lie SA , Larsen JP ((2004) ) Cigarette smoking in Parkinson’s disease: Influence on disease progression. Mov Disord 19: , 1087–1092.

[32] 

Kandinov B , Giladi N , Korczyn AD ((2007) ) The effect of cigarette smoking, tea, and coffee consumption on the progression of Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 13: , 243–245.

[33] 

Hernan MA , Zhang SM , Rueda-deCastro AM , Colditz GA , Speizer FE , Ascherio A ((2001) ) Cigarette smoking and the incidence of Parkinson’s disease in two prospective studies. Ann Neurol 50: , 780–786.

[34] 

Yang F , Pedersen NL , Ye W , Liu Z , Norberg M , Forsgren L , Trolle Lagerros Y , Bellocco R , Alfredsson L , Knutsson A , Jansson JH , Wennberg P , Galanti MR , Lager ACJ , Araghi M , Lundberg M , Magnusson C , Wirdefeldt K ((2017) ) Moist smokeless tobacco (Snus) use and risk of Parkinson’s disease. Int J Epidemiol 46: , 872–880.

[35] 

Villafane G , Thiriez C , Audureau E , Straczek C , Kerschen P , Cormier-Dequaire F , Van Der Gucht A , Gurruchaga JM , Quere-Carne M , Evangelista E , Paul M , Defer G , Damier P , Remy P , Itti E , Fenelon G ((2018) ) High-dose transdermal nicotine in Parkinson’s disease patients: A randomized, open-label, blinded-endpoint evaluation phase 2 study. Eur J Neurol 25: , 120–128.

[36] 

Oertel W , Müller H , Shade-Brittinger C , Kamp C , Balthasar K , Articus K , Brinkman M , Venuto C , Unger M , Eggert K , Vadasz D , Kieburtz K , Boyd J ((2018) ) The NIC-PD-study –A randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind, multi-centre trial to assess the disease-modifying potential of transdermal nicotine in early Parkinson’s disease in Germany and N. America [abstract].https://www.mdsabstracts.org/abstract/the-nic-pd-study-a-randomized-placebo-controlled-double-blind-multi-centre-trial-to-assess-the-disease-modifying-potential-of-transdermal-nicotine-in-early-parkinsons-disease-in-g/. Mov Disord 33 (Suppl 2): .

[37] 

Wirdefeldt K , Gatz M , Pawitan Y , Pedersen NL ((2005) ) Risk and protective factors for Parkinson’s disease: A study in Swedish twins. Ann Neurol 57: , 27–33.

[38] 

Vandenbroucke JP , von Elm E , Altman DG , Gotzsche PC , Mulrow CD , Pocock SJ , Poole C , Schlesselman JJ , Egger M ((2014) ) Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): Explanation and elaboration. Int J Surg 12: , 1500–1524.

[39] 

Ludvigsson JF , Andersson E , Ekbom A , Feychting M , Kim JL , Reuterwall C , Heurgren M , Olausson PO ((2011) ) External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health 11: , 450.

[40] 

Ludvigsson JF , Svedberg P , Olén O , Bruze G , Neovius M ((2019) ) The longitudinal integrated database for health insurance and labour market studies (LISA) and its use in medical research. Eur J Epidemiol 34: , 423–437.

[41] 

Tanner CM , Goldman SM , Aston DA , Ottman R , Ellenberg J , Mayeux R , Langston JW ((2002) ) Smoking and Parkinson’s disease in twins. Neurology 58: , 581–588.

[42] 

Kauhanen L , Lakka HM , Lynch JW , Kauhanen J ((2006) ) Social disadvantages in childhood and risk of all-cause death and cardiovascular disease in later life: A comparison of historical and retrospective childhood information. Int J Epidemiol 35: , 962–968.