Searching for just a few words should be enough to get started. If you need to make more complex queries, use the tips below to guide you.
Article type: Research Article
Authors: Dion, Catherinea | Arias, Franchescab | Amini, Shawnac | Davis, Randalld | Penney, Danae | Libon, David J.f | Price, Catherine C.a; c; *
Affiliations: [a] Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA | [b] Aging Brain Center at Hebrew Senior Life, Harvard University, Boston, MA, USA | [c] Department of Anesthesiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA | [d] Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA | [e] Department of Neurology, Lahey Clinic Medical Center, Burlington, MA, USA | [f] Department of Geriatrics, Gerontology, and Psychology, Rowan University, Glassboro, NJ, USA
Correspondence: [*] Correspondence to: Catherine C. Price, PhD, ABPP/CN, Associate Professor, Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, College of PHHP, P.O. Box 100165, Gainesville, FL 32605-6617, USA. Tel.: +1 352 273 5929; E-mail: [email protected].
Abstract: Background:A digital version of the clock drawing test (dCDT) provides new latency and graphomotor behavioral measurements. These variables have yet to be validated with external neuropsychological domains in non-demented adults. Objective:The current investigation reports on cognitive constructs associated with selected dCDT latency and graphomotor variables and compares performances between individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and non-MCI peers. Methods:202 non-demented older adults (age 68.79 ± 6.18, 46% female, education years 16.02 ± 2.70) completed the dCDT and a comprehensive neuropsychological protocol. dCDT variables of interest included: total completion time (TCT), pre-first hand latency (PFHL), post-clock face latency (PCFL), and clock face area (CFA). We also explored variables of percent time drawing (i.e., ‘ink time’) versus percent time not drawing (i.e., ‘think time’). Neuropsychological domains of interest included processing speed, working memory, language, and declarative memory. Results:Adjusting for age and premorbid cognitive reserve metrics, command TCT positively correlated with multiple cognitive domains; PFHL and PCFL negatively associated with worse performance on working memory and processing speed tests. For Copy, TCT, PCFL, and PFHL negatively correlated with processing speed, and CFA negatively correlated with language. Between-group analyses show MCI participants generated slower command TCT, produced smaller CFA, and required more command ‘think’ (% Think) than ‘ink’ (% Ink) time. Conclusion:Command dCDT variables of interest were primarily processing speed and working memory dependent. MCI participants showed dCDT differences relative to non-MCI peers, suggesting the dCDT may assist with classification. Results document cognitive construct validation to digital metrics of clock drawing.
Keywords: Attention, cognition, cognitive aging, neuropsychology, processing speed
DOI: 10.3233/JAD-191089
Journal: Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 75, no. 1, pp. 73-83, 2020
IOS Press, Inc.
6751 Tepper Drive
Clifton, VA 20124
USA
Tel: +1 703 830 6300
Fax: +1 703 830 2300
[email protected]
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to [email protected]
IOS Press
Nieuwe Hemweg 6B
1013 BG Amsterdam
The Netherlands
Tel: +31 20 688 3355
Fax: +31 20 687 0091
[email protected]
For editorial issues, permissions, book requests, submissions and proceedings, contact the Amsterdam office [email protected]
Inspirees International (China Office)
Ciyunsi Beili 207(CapitaLand), Bld 1, 7-901
100025, Beijing
China
Free service line: 400 661 8717
Fax: +86 10 8446 7947
[email protected]
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to [email protected]
如果您在出版方面需要帮助或有任何建, 件至: [email protected]